Body Image 2 (2005) 383–393 www.elsevier.com/locate/bodyimage Male physical attractiveness in Britain and Malaysia: A cross-cultural study Viren Swami a,*, Martin J. Tovée b a Department of Psychology, University College of London, 26, Bedford Way, London WC1E 6BT, UK b Department of Psychology, Newcastle University, UK Received 8 May 2005; received in revised form 5 August 2005; accepted 6 August 2005 Abstract Three purported cues to perceived male physical attractiveness are the waist-to-chest ratio (WCR), body mass index (BMI) and the waist-to-hip ratio (WHR). This study examined the relative contribution of each cue in several socio-economically distinct populations. Ninety-five female participants from Britain and Malaysia were asked to rate a set of images of real men with known WCR, BMI and WHR. The results showed clear differences along a gradient of socio-economic development. In urban settings, WCR was the primary component of attractiveness ratings, with BMI playing a smaller role and WHR not reaching significance. In the rural setting, BMI was the primary predictor of attractiveness, with WCR playing a more minor role and WHR not reaching significance. In general, urban participants were more reliant on body shape and chose a relatively slim figure with an ‘inverted triangle’ shape; rural participants were more reliant on body weight and chose a heavier figure with a less triangular shape. These findings are discussed in terms of evolutionary psychological explanations of mate selection and sociological theories that emphasise the effect of resource scarcity on preferences for body shapes and sizes. # 2005 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. Keywords: Physical attractiveness; Waist-to-chest ratio; Body mass index; Cross-cultural Introduction Although a great deal of the research concerning physical attractiveness has focussed on the female body, researchers are increasingly paying attention to masculinity and the male body (e.g., Garrett, 1992; Stam, 1998). Much of this research has employed an evolutionary psychological perspective and considers * Corresponding author. Tel.: +44 207 6795302. E-mail address: [email protected] (V. Swami). different traits to be an indicator of genetic variability. This explanation assumes that a reliable connection exists between body attractiveness and male quality, that male attractiveness is an indicator or some component of fitness such as health and vigour and that females detect and use this indicator for choosing a mate (Shackelford et al., 2000; Singh, 1995) The most obvious case of sexually selected characters in humans concerns features such as beards and body shape that differ conspicuously between the sexes (Barber, 1995). Thus, it has been argued that 1740-1445/$ – see front matter # 2005 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2005.08.001 384 V. Swami, M.J. Tovée / Body Image 2 (2005) 383–393 men with dominance- and masculinity-related facial and body characteristics are considered most physically and sexually attractive. Testosterone and areas of the body indexing testosterone are suggested to play key roles, as dominant males have higher testosterone levels (Ehrenkantz, Bliss, & Sheard, 1974). It has also been suggested that aspects of male body build, particularly the upper torso, might be sexually selected. The shoulders of men, their upper body musculature and biceps are all more developed than in women, even when differences in stature are accounted for (Ross & Ward, 1982), and these characteristics are influenced by testosterone levels (Björntorp, 1987). Using silhouettes as stimuli, a number of studies have shown that females tend to prefer a moderately developed male torso than extremely muscular physiques (Barber, 1995). However, most of these studies have not looked explicitly at male bodily physique, but have used line drawings to focus on the waist-to-hip ratio (WHR; the ratio of the width of the waist to the width of the hips). These studies report a preferred WHR of 0.90–0.95 (Furnham, Tan, & McManus, 1997; Henss, 1995; Lynch & Zellner, 1999; Singh, 1995). However, more recent research using photographic stimuli shows that while the WHR, body mass index (BMI) and waist-to-chest ratio (WCR) are all significant contributors to male attractiveness, WCR was the principal determinant and accounted for 56% of the variance (Maisey, Vale, Cornelissen, & Tovée, 1999; see also Fan, Dai, Liu, & Wu, 2005). By contrast, BMI accounted for only 12.7% of the variance and the WHR was not a significant predictor of attractiveness. Maisey et al. (1999) concluded that women’s ratings of male attractiveness can be explained by simple physical characteristics that measure body shape (in particular the WCR). Women are said to prefer men whose torso has an ‘inverted triangle’ shape, that is, a narrow waist and a broad chest and shoulders, which is consistent with physical strength and muscle development in the upper body. This finding is comparable with other studies using line drawings which show that women prefer men with a ‘V-shape’ (wider shoulders than chest, which was again wider than the hips; Frederick & Haselton, 2003; Furnham & Radley, 1989; Lavrakas, 1975). In opposition to evolutionary psychological explanations, however, it has been suggested that society has expectations for ideal male body shapes (HesseBiber, 1996; Murray, Touyz, & Beumont, 1996) and that males increasingly compare their bodies to idealised media and cultural images (Davis & Katzman, 1997; Heinberg, Thompson, & Stormer, 1995; McCreary & Sasse, 2000). Although gender differences emerge in attitudes toward cultural ideals of attractiveness, with women more motivated to conform to these ideals than men (Cash & Hicks, 1990; Muth & Cash, 1997), socio-cultural pressures concerning male body image seem to be on the increase. For example, one content analysis found a consistency in the V-shaped standard of male bodily attractiveness presented in US men’s magazines between 1960 and 1992 (Petrie et al., 1996). In a more recent study, Leit, Pope, and Gray (2001) examined centrefold models in Playgirl from 1973 to 1997, and found that the cultural norm for the ideal male body has become increasingly muscular, especially in the 1990s. If judgements of attractiveness are an innate preference, as evolutionary psychology argues, then it might be suggested that these preferences should be consistent across cultures. Although there is now a growing body of evidence examining body type preferences for the male body, the literature examining these preferences cross-culturally remains limited (Cash & Pruzinsky, 2002). Using an undergraduate sample of Caucasian and Asian–American students, Mintz and Kashubeck (1999) found that males aspired for a large, muscular cultural ideal that does not differ between ethnic groups. However, while Mintz and Kashubeck (1999) explored satisfaction with specific body parts, they did not specifically investigate the interaction between ethnicity and gender on overall body figure preference. A more recent study suggests that Asian–American men are more invested in developing a large, muscular body (Barnett, Keel, & Conoscenti, 2001), but to date few studies have examined male physical attractiveness cross-nationally, using female participants. This study therefore set out to explore judgements of male physical attractiveness among groups of female observers from different socio-economic and cultural backgrounds. Previously, we reported that, because of unequal socio-economic transformation in recent decades, Malaysia furnishes a natural laboratory in V. Swami, M.J. Tovée / Body Image 2 (2005) 383–393 which to examine preferences for physical attractiveness (Swami & Tovée, 2005a). In different parts of Malaysia, people live in industrialised and rural environments, and this allows us to explore the effect of industrialisation on standards of male attractiveness. Participants were, therefore, recruited from Britain (a relatively modern and developed sample), Kuala Lumpur (a culturally modern area of Malaysia) and from Sabah (a rural, relatively under-developed area of Malaysia). Importantly, the two Malaysian samples were ethnically similar to one another, whereas the two urban samples were culturally similar, reflecting shared cultural values of modernity. Methods Participants The participants of this study were recruited from two countries, Britain and Malaysia, there being three groups reflecting a gradient of socio-economic development from industrialised (Britain and Kuala Lumpur) to rural (Kota Belud outskirts). The first group consisted of 30 female participants recruited from Kuala Lumpur, the largest city in Malaysia with a population of about 2 million. As the capital city of Malaysia and an independent federal territory, Kuala Lumpur is the heart of the Malaysian economy with a GDP per capita of about US$ 8000 and a low unemployment rate (2.6%; Government of Malaysia, 2001). Compared with East Malaysia, which is about 50% rural, Kuala Lumpur is a modern city that has witnessed a noticeable rise in clinical eating disorders (e.g., Hsien-Jin, 2000). Although a number of different ethnic groups populate the city, we only recruited participants of Malay ancestry. The second group consisted of 28 female participants recruited from rural villages on the 385 outskirts of the small town of Kota Belud, in the state of Sabah in East Malaysia. Sabah is one of Malaysia’s least developed states, with a GDP per capita of about US$ 2400 and a high unemployment rate at 5.6%. In 1999, the average monthly household income for Sabah was RM 1905 (about US$ 500), the lowest in Malaysia (Government of Malaysia, 2001). Most participants in this group were paddy farmers, depending on the crop for their livelihood. A small number also worked as temporary labourers in Kota Belud. Although Sabah has witnessed increasing urbanisation in recent years (including the dispersal of urban development and new highway construction through rural areas), the area around Kota Belud has retained its essentially rural character. All villages in the area of study had a permanent supply of electricity and water, though sources of mass media were often restricted to communal televisions (regulated state channels) and print matter (local newspapers). Again, only participants of Malay ancestry were included in the group. Both Malaysian groups were compared with a sample of 37 Caucasian females who were born and raised in urban Britain. The age ranges and means of the different groups are summarised in Table 1. There were no significant differences in the mean ages of the different groups (one-way ANOVA, F(2,94) = 0.02, p > 0.05). There were also no significant differences in the body mass indices of participants in the different groups (one-way ANOVA, F(2,94) = 1.04, p > 0.05). Materials Participants in each group were asked to rate blackand-white images of 50 real men in front view. To generate the images, consenting men were previously (Maisey et al., 1999) videoed standing in a set pose at a standard distance, wearing tight grey leotards and Table 1 Participants’ age and BMI ranges, means and standard deviations Group Britain Kuala Lumpur Sabah Age BMI Range Mean Standard deviation Range Mean Standard deviation 19–39 18–38 20–38 24.70 24.43 24.67 6.02 5.32 4.88 15.59–30.82 16.65–27.83 16.73–31.64 21.03 22.18 22.00 3.21 3.12 4.28 386 V. Swami, M.J. Tovée / Body Image 2 (2005) 383–393 Fig. 1. Example of male images used in the experiment. leggings in front view. Images were then framegrabbed and stored as 24-bit images (see Fig. 1). The use of high-resolution photographic images is more realistic than the line drawings used to date, but it should be noted that a two-dimensional image may not capture all the visual cues available from a threedimensional image seen from the same viewing point (DeSoto & Kopp, 2003). However, a recent study which compared the ratings of two-dimensional photographs with ratings of movie clips of the same bodies rotated through 3608 found no differences (Smith, Cornelissen, & Tovée, 2005), suggesting that two-dimensional photographs can capture much of the visual information available in three-dimensional images. Although the images were derived from men of British origin, every effort was taken to ensure that ethnicity would not be a factor. To this end, the heads of the men in the images were obscured, so that they could not be identified and so that facial attractiveness would not be a factor in subjects’ ratings. The images were also presented in black-and-white to minimise the impact of skin colour. The waist-to-chest ratio, waist-to-hip ratio and body mass index were measured directly from each of the volunteers themselves and not the pictures. To ensure there was an equal range or variation of BMI, WCR and WHR within the image set, these characteristics were first measured from a sample of 214 men (see Maisey et al., 1999). The 50 images representing a range of standard deviation 1.7 either side of the mean of the distribution of BMI, WCR and WHR in the sample were then chosen for the final image set. The men in the final set varied in WCR from 0.69 to 0.89, in WHR from 0.83 to 0.98 and BMI from 18.94 kg/m2 to 28.07 kg/m2. For this experiment, the images of men were printed on sheets of A4 paper, so that each image covered the entire page. This was done so as to facilitate replication of the study in all locations. Participants were presented with a booklet to record their ratings, where the first page consisted of brief instructions and a worked example of a rating, and where the final page requested participants’ demographic details (age, gender, ethnicity, weight and height). Other pages in the booklet provided a 9-point horizontal Likert scale, which appeared below the question ‘How physically attractive is the person in the photograph?’ and on which participants were asked to record their ratings. Procedure All participants were tested individually, with the only difference in procedure between the different settings being the language used. While the questionnaire was in English for the British group, instructions were translated into Malay for the Malaysian samples. A back translation of the instructions by an independent translator certified the validity of the questionnaire across conditions. Within the image set, individual images were presented in a randomised order, and subjects were presented with the entire set twice. This was done to make participants aware of the range of variability of body features represented in the images in the first run through, and to encourage participants to use the whole set of attractiveness ratings from 1 (least attractive) to 9 (most attractive). Participants were V. Swami, M.J. Tovée / Body Image 2 (2005) 383–393 387 Results Fig. 2. Plots of attractiveness as functions of WCR. Each point represents the 50 attractiveness judgements made by participants. Regression lines (solid lines) and their 95% confidence levels (dotted lines) are superimposed. (a) Britain; (b) Kuala Lumpur; (c) Sabah. only asked to rate the images according to the leading question on the second run through. The entire procedure took approximately 40 min to complete for each participant. A multiple polynomial regression was used to model the contributions of WCR, WHR and BMI to the attractiveness ratings. Fig. 2 shows plots of attractiveness ratings as a function of WCR for all three groups, with all sets being significantly explained by WCR (Britain, t(49) = 7.11, p < 0.001; Kuala Lumpur, t(49) = 7.57, p < 0.001; Sabah, t(49) = 2.87, p < 0.001). It is clear that there is a strong correlation of attractiveness and WCR, with even small changes in WCR significantly altering the attractiveness rating of the male body. Fig. 3 shows the corresponding relationship between attractiveness and BMI, with all sets being significantly explained by BMI (Britain, t(49) = 2.32, p < 0.05; Kuala Lumpur, t(49) = 2.45, p < 0.05; Sabah, t(49) = 5.89, p < 0.001). It can be seen from these figures that the relationship between BMI and attractiveness is non-linear. That is, increases or decreases in BMI either side of the peak of the curve reduce the attractiveness rating. Finally, Fig. 4 shows the relationship between attractiveness and WHR. There was no significant effect of WHR on any of the groups (Britain, t(49) = 0.22, p > 0.05; Kuala Lumpur, t(49) = 0.32, p > 0.05; Sabah, t(49) = 0.91, p > 0.05), suggesting that the WHR does not have an effect on the attractiveness ratings of participants. There are a large number of functions that could be used to model these data. Following Maisey et al. (1999), we chose the simplest approach possible, which was to include second-order terms in a multiple regression model, to estimate the variance of attractiveness ratings explained by WCR, WHR and BMI. Determining what order polynomial to fit to the data was considered in earlier publications (Maisey et al., 1999), and we settled on a second-order polynomial for BMI because it balances the amount of variance accounted for with the simplest possible regression model. In addition, there appears little justification in the psychological literature for fitting a more complex function (Altman, 1991). The model, run separately for the different groups, was: y ¼ a þ b1 x1 þ b2 x2 þ b3 x3 þ b4 x4 þ e where y is the attractiveness rating, a the intercept, x1 the BMI, x2 the BMP, x3 the WCR, x4 the WHR and e is random error. 388 V. Swami, M.J. Tovée / Body Image 2 (2005) 383–393 Fig. 3. Plots of attractiveness as functions of BMI. Each point represents the 50 attractiveness judgements made by participants. Regression lines (solid lines) and their 95% confidence levels (dotted lines) are superimposed. (a) Britain; (b) Kuala Lumpur; (c) Sabah. Fig. 4. Plots of attractiveness as functions of WHR. Each point represents the 50 attractiveness judgements made by participants. Regression lines (solid lines) and their 95% confidence levels (dotted lines) are superimposed. (a) Britain; (b) Kuala Lumpur; (c) Sabah. The total variance explained by this model for the relationship between WCR and attractiveness ratings is shown in Table 2, and is dramatically different between the groups. While the relationship accounted for about 50% of the variance in both urban groups (Britain and Kuala Lumpur), it accounted for only 24.1% in the rural group. This suggests that whereas both urban groups use the WCR as the primary V. Swami, M.J. Tovée / Body Image 2 (2005) 383–393 389 Table 2 Total variance explained by WCR, BMI and WHR and peak attractiveness Group Britain Kuala Lumpur Sabah Variance Correlation with ratings (r) WCR BMI WHR WCR BMI WHR 53.7 56.9 24.1 19.8 15.3 53.2 0.1 0.1 0.4 0.821 0.820 +0.552 0.570 0.510 +0.775 0.351 0.312 +0.219 predictor of male bodily attractiveness, the rural group does not. In addition, the gradient of this relationship is similar for participants in Britain (23.02) and Kuala Lumpur (24.18), with increasing attractiveness as the bodies become more V-shaped (that is, the WCR is lower). However, the gradient is strikingly different for participants in Sabah (+7.39), suggesting that more tubular bodies (that is, the WCR is higher and closer to 1.0) are considered as more attractive. To test whether this difference was statistically significant, we carried out a series of dummy regressions (Tukey, 1977). That is, in order to allow the relationships between the response variable (attractiveness rating) and the explanatory variable (WCR) to differ for the different groups, we introduced a dummy variable into the regression model (Hays, 1988). This allows us to model a regression for two observer groups and determine whether they are significantly different. Using this analysis, we found that the rural group was significantly different from both other groups ( p < 0.001). The British and Kuala Lumpur groups were not significantly different from each other. This was further confirmed by the correlations between WCR and attractiveness ratings: for the British group, r = 0.821; for the Kuala Lumpur group, r = 0.820; for the Sabahan groups, r = +0.552 (see Table 2). This basic pattern is reversed when the relationship between BMI and attractiveness is considered (see Table 2). For both urban groups, the relationship accounted for less than 20% of the variance, suggesting that BMI is a relatively weaker predictor of male bodily attractiveness than WCR. For the Sabahan group, however, BMI accounted for over 50% of the variance, which suggests that BMI is a considerably stronger determinant of bodily attractiveness than WCR in this group. This is borne out by the correlations between BMI and attractiveness ratings: for the British group, r = 0.570; for the Peak attractiveness WCR gradient 20.86, SD 0.97 20.62, SD 1.11 24.09, SD 2.41 27.30 26.68 +15.73 Kuala Lumpur group, r = 0.510; for the Sabahan groups, r = +0.775 (see Table 2). From Fig. 3, it is noticeable that the attractiveness versus BMI function has shifted in the rural group (that is, the peak or ‘ideal’ BMI value may differ across the groups). To explore this possibility, we followed Tovée, Maisey, Emery, and Cornelissen (1999) in fitting second-order polynomials for BMI to the attractiveness ratings made by all participants in each group, allowing the BMI at peak attractiveness to be calculated for each participant (see Table 2). There were overall significant differences between the ratings made by participants from the different groups (one-way ANOVA, F(2,94) = 57.02, p < 0.001). To see where these differences lay, a post hoc Tukey HSD was carried out on the data. The peak BMI values from the Sabahan group were significantly higher (24.09 kg/m2) from both the Kuala Lumpur (20.62 kg/m2) and British (20.86 kg/m2) groups, which were not significantly different from each other. Discussion The results of this study show that a woman’s rating of male bodily attractiveness can be explained by simple physical characteristics, in particular the WCR and BMI. However, there are clear cross-cultural differences in the way these characteristics are used. In urban settings in Malaysia and Britain, the WCR is the primary component of attractiveness ratings, suggesting that upper body shape is more important for male attractiveness than body mass. Women prefer men whose torso has an ‘inverted triangle’shape, which is consistent with previous findings (Fan et al., 2005; Frederick & Haselton, 2003; Furnham & Radley, 1989; Lavrakas, 1975; Maisey et al., 1999); the BMI of the male body is comparatively unimportant. This is distinguishable from investigations of female attractiveness, which 390 V. Swami, M.J. Tovée / Body Image 2 (2005) 383–393 show that body weight is the primary predictor of attractiveness ratings (Fan, Liu, Wu, & Dai, 2004; Swami & Tovée, 2005a; Tovée et al., 1999). By contrast, BMI is the primary cue for male attractiveness in rural Malaysia, with body shape (as measured by the WCR and WHR) playing a comparatively minor role. The preference among rural participants for heavier men is combined with a preference for a more tubular body shape (that is, changing upper body shape has less of an effect on attractiveness in the rural group, and a less curvaceous shape is regarded as relatively more attractive in the rural group than in the other observer groups). This set of findings is striking given existing cross-cultural evidence suggesting that Asian– American men, like their Caucasian counterparts, are invested in developing a large, muscular body (Barnett et al., 2001; Mintz & Kashubeck, 1999). Rather, when ratings are elicited from rural contexts, body mass and not shape is the primary cue for male physical attractiveness. Some evolutionary psychologists have suggested that judgements of attractiveness are an innate preference, and that consequently such preferences should be consistent across cultures (Singh, 1995). However, the results of this study show that cultural differences can exist in the perception of male attractiveness, and that these differences may be linked with socio-economic status (see Swami & Tovée, 2005a). Cross-cultural research (primarily regarding female physical attractiveness) has revealed differing perceptions of attractiveness and healthy body sizes (e.g., Furnham & Alibhai, 1983; Furnham & Baguma, 1994; Miller & Pumariega, 2001; Powers, 1980; Swami, Caprario, Tovée, & Furnham, in press; Wetsman & Marlowe, 1999; Yu & Shepard, 1998), with increasing modernisation and economic liberalisation resulting in the acquisition of slimmer body mass preferences. In most traditional, non-industrialised settings, body fat is believed to be an indicator of wealth and prosperity (McGarvey, 1991), with obesity as a symbol of economic success and sexual capacity (Ghannam, 1997; Nasser, 1988; Rudovsky, 1974). Furthermore, there is often a positive relationship between increased socio-economic status and body weight: only high-status individuals would have been able to put on body weight (Brown & Konner, 1987), as it would have been advantageous to be able to store excess food as fat in times of surplus. The reliance on and preference for larger BMIs among rural women in this study may therefore reflect such relationships. Until recently, this pattern linking resource availability (as indicated by socio-economic status) and body weight lacked an obvious psychological mechanism. Nelson and Morrison (2005), however, proposed an implicit psychological mechanism based on the situational influence of environmental conditions. They argue that the consequence of collective resource scarcity is that individual members of a society in which resources are scarce are likely to lack resources themselves. They further argue that the affective and physiological states associated with individual-level resource availability provide implicit information about collective resource availability, and that this then plays a role in the construction of preferences. In a series of studies, Nelson and Morrison (2005) tested this hypothesis by manipulating people’s financial satisfaction or hunger (both these being proxies for personal resources in industrialised societies) and measuring their preferences for potential romantic partners. Their studies confirmed that implicit cues to resource availability influence preference for potential mates: financially dissatisfied and hungry men preferred a heavier mate than did financially satisfied men or satiated men, respectively (see also Swami & Tovée, 2005b). Although their studies only examined male preferences for female body weight, the results of the present study suggest that their findings may hold (although in different directions) when female perceptions of male attractiveness are considered. Importantly, industrialised societies appear to promulgate different expectations for ideal male body shapes (Andersen & DiDomenico, 1992; Frederick, Fessler, & Haselton, 2005; Hesse-Biber, 1996; Leit et al., 2001; Murray et al., 1996; Peixoto Labre, 2002), which women may idealise. The preference for a large, muscular and mesomorphic body type in industrialised settings appears to develop at a very young age (Collins & Plahn, 1988; Lerner, 1969, 1972; McCreary & Sasse, 2000; Staffieri, 1967) and has been linked with media use and exposure (Morry & Staska, 2001). In a recent study, for example, Botta (2003) surveyed US college students to test the extent to which reading fashion, sports, health or fitness V. Swami, M.J. Tovée / Body Image 2 (2005) 383–393 magazines is related to body image and eating disorders. Results indicated that reading was linked to increased muscularity, which means that the more time men spent reading, the more likely they were to be committed to being muscular. The absence of a strong preference for muscular, Vshaped bodies among rural participants in the present study lends credence to the view that such an ideal is a culturally influenced phenomenon. Although we have no explicit measures of exposure to media images of male bodies, our ethnographic observation corroborates the relative lack of exposure to Western media images among rural Sabahans. Although some of the women in this study have televisions in their homes, this is restricted to regulated state-run channels. In addition, local print media typically do not feature images of men or masculine figures. In Kota Belud, which women may visit occasionally, there are several eating establishments that broadcast foreign channels, and Western magazines are available in some shops. However, for the most part, advertisements are Malaysian and the most prominent reading materials are Sabahan newspapers. In addition to media exposure, other cultural factors may play a role in explaining the attitudes of females about male physical attractiveness. Emerging evidence, for example, highlights personal and socio-cultural factors, especially parental and peer influences (Field et al., 2001; Irving, Wall, NeumarkSztainer, & Story, 2002; Ricciardelli & McCabe, 2001a). One possibility is that, in most industrialised settings like Britain and Kuala Lumpur, women have rapidly achieved parity with men in many aspects of life, leaving men with only their bodies as a distinguishing source of masculinity (Faludi, 1999; Leit et al., 2001; Nixon, 1996). Images of muscular, fit and toned men thus represent men seeking to embody the physical strength, hardness and power associated with the traditional muscular ideal, signalling distance from traditional cultural ideas about feminity. The limitations of this paper include the fact the stimuli employed attempted to measure BMI and WCR at the possible expense of ecological validity. For example, the range of body sizes and shapes used in this study may not have been representational of Malaysian men. Future studies should ensure as far as possible that stimuli used are suited to local cultural and demographic conditions. This limitation notwith- 391 standing, the fact that the more urban Malaysian sample has come to agree so closely with the British sample and differ so notably from the rural Malaysian sample offers a suggestion that modernisation can bring about quite rapid changes in attractiveness perception. Because Sabahan women live in a relatively rural setting characterised by few media images of male bodies, they may be ‘buffered’ from the more muscular, ‘inverted triangle’ shape prevalent in industrialised settings. In other words, the preference for specific body shapes in men appears to result from exposure to modernity and related media images of muscular bodies. Insofar as Malaysian males seek to adopt larger and more muscular bodies, a phenomenon already prevalent in the West (Abell & Richards, 1996; Furnham & Calnan, 1998; McCabe & Ricciardelli, 2001; Ricciardelli & McCabe, 2001b), it may be expected that they would be at particular risk for developing body image disturbances. Acknowledgements The first author was supported by an Overseas Research Student Award from Universities UK. The second author’s research is supported by Wellcome Trust funding through the JIF initiative. We would like to acknowledge the contributions of Dr. Dorothy Einon, Prof. Adrian Furnham, Dr. Alaistair McClelland and Prof. Thomas Cash, as well as four reviewers who made comments on earlier versions of this study. References Abell, S. C., & Richards, M. H. (1996). The relationship between body shape satisfaction and self-esteem: An investigation of gender and class difference. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 25, 691–703. Altman, D. G. (1991). Practical statistics for medical research. London: Chapman & Hall. Andersen, A. E., & DiDomenico, L. (1992). Diet versus shape content of popular male and female magazines: A dose–response relationship to the incidence of eating disorders? International Journal of Eating Disorders, 11, 283–287. Barber, N. (1995). The evolutionary psychology of physical attractiveness: Sexual selection and human morphology. Ethology and Socio-biology, 16, 395–424. 392 V. Swami, M.J. Tovée / Body Image 2 (2005) 383–393 Barnett, H. L., Keel, P. K., & Conoscenti, L. (2001). Body type preferences in Asian and Caucasian students. Sex Roles, 45, 867–878. Björntorp, P. (1987). Fat cell distribution and metabolism. In R. J. Wurtman, & J. J. Wurtman (Eds.), Human obesity (pp. 66–72). New York: New York Academy of Sciences. Botta, R. A. (2003). For your health? The relationship between magazine reading and adolescents’ body image and eating disturbances. Sex Roles, 48, 389–399. Brown, P., & Konner, M. J. (1987). An anthropological perspective of obesity. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 499, 29. Cash, T. F., & Hicks, K. L. (1990). Being fat versus thinking fat: Relationships with body image, eating behaviours, and wellbeing. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14, 237–341. Cash, T. F., & Pruzinsky, T. (2002). Future challenges for body image theory, research, and clinical practice. In T. F. Cash, & T. Pruzinsky (Eds.), Body image: A handbook of theory, research, and clinical practice (pp. 509–516). New York: The Guilford Press. Collins, J. K., & Plahn, M. R. (1988). Recognition accuracy, stereotypic preference, aversion and subjective judgement of body appearance in adolescents and young adults. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 17, 317–332. Davis, C., & Katzman, M. (1997). Charting new territory: Body esteem, weight satisfaction, depression and self-esteem among Chinese males and females in Hong Kong. Sex Roles, 36, 449– 459. DeSoto, M. C., & Kopp, K. J. (2003). Predicting female attractiveness: A second look at thinness and waist-to-hip ratio.. Sexualities, Evolution & Gender, 5, 83–88. Ehrenkantz, J., Bliss, E., & Sheard, M. H. (1974). Plasma testosterone: Correlation with aggressive behaviour and social dominance in man. Psychosomatic Medicine, 36, 469–475. Faludi, S. (1999). Stiffed: The betrayal of the American man. New York: W Morrow & Co. Fan, J. T., Dai, W., Liu, F., & Wu, J. (2005). Visual perception of male body attractiveness. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 272, 219–226. Fan, J. T., Liu, F., Wu, J., & Dai, W. (2004). Visual perception of female physical attractiveness. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 271, 347–352. Field, A. E., Carmago, C. A., Taylor, C. B., Berkey, C. S., Roberts, S. B., & Coldizt, G. A. (2001). Peer, parent and media influences on the development of weight concerns and frequent dieting among preadolescent and adolescent girls and boys. Paediatrics, 107, 54–60. Frederick, D. A., Fessler, D. M. T., & Haselton, M. G. (2005). Do representations of male muscularity differ in men’s and women’s magazines? Body Image, 2, 81–86. Frederick, D. A., & Haselton, M. G. (2003). Muscularity as a communicative signal. Paper presentation at the International Communications Association. Furnham, A., & Alibhai, N. (1983). Cross-cultural differences in the perception of female body-shapes. Psychological Medicine, 13, 829–837. Furnham, A., & Baguma, P. (1994). Cross-cultural differences in the evaluation of male and female body shapes. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 15, 81–89. Furnham, A., & Calnan, A. (1998). Eating disturbance, self-esteem, reasons for exercising and body weight dissatisfaction in adolescent males. European Eating Disorders Review, 6, 58– 72. Furnham, A. F., & Radley, S. (1989). Sex differences in the perceptions of male and female body shapes. Personality and Individual Differences, 10, 653–662. Furnham, A., Tan, T., & McManus, C. (1997). Waist-to-hip ratio and preferences for body shape: A replication and extension. Personal and Individual Differences, 22, 539–549. Garrett, C. (1992). Anorexia nervosa as personal and social ritual. Working papers in women’s studies, number 6. Nepean, Australia: Women’s Research Centre, University of Western Sydney. Ghannam, F. (1997). Fertile, plump and strong: The social construction of female body in low income Cairo. Monographs in reproductive health number 3, Cairo: Population Council Regional Office for West Asia and North Africa. Government of Malaysia. (2001). The eight Malaysia plan, 2001– 2005. Putrajaya, Malaysia: Economic Planning Unit, Prime Minister’s Department. Hays, W. L. (1988). Statistics (4th ed.). Florida: Hold Rinehart & Winston Inc. Heinberg, L. J., Thompson, J. K., & Stormer, S. (1995). Development and validation of the Sociocultural Attitudes Towards Appearance Questionnaire. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 17, 81–89. Henss, R. (1995). Waist-to-hip ratio and attractiveness. Replication and extension. Personality and Individual Differences, 19, 479– 488. Hesse-Biber, S. (1996). Am I thin enough yet? The cult of thinness and the commercialisation of identity New York: Oxford University Press. Hsien-Jin, T. (2000). Sociocultural and developmental theories as explanations of body image dissatisfaction amongst secondary school girls. Malaysian Journal of Psychiatry, 8, 15–21. Irving, L. M., Wall, M., Neumark-Sztainer, D., & Story, M. (2002). Steroid use among adolescents: Findings from Project EAT. Journal of Adolescent Health, 30, 243–252. Lavrakas, P. J. (1975). Female preferences for male physique. Journal of Research in Personality, 9, 324–334. Leit, R. A., Pope, H. G., Jr., & Gray, J. J. (2001). Cultural expectations of muscularity in men: The evolution of Playgirl centrefolds. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 29, 90 93. Lerner, R. M. (1969). The development of stereotyped expectancies of body build–behaviour relations. Developmental Psychology, 40, 137–141. Lerner, R. M. (1972). ‘Richness’ analyses of body build stereotype development. Development Psychology, 7, 219. Lynch, S. M., & Zellner, D. A. (1999). Figure preferences in two generations of men: The use of figure drawings illustrating differences in muscle mass. Sex Roles, 40, 833–843. Maisey, D. M., Vale, E. L. E., Cornelissen, P. L., & Tovée, M. J. (1999). Characteristics of male attractiveness for women. Lancet, 353, 1500. McCabe, M. P., & Ricciardelli, L. A. (2001). Body image and body change techniques among young adolescent boys. European Eating Disorders Review, 9, 1–13. V. Swami, M.J. Tovée / Body Image 2 (2005) 383–393 McCreary, D. R., & Sasse, D. K. (2000). An exploration of the drive for muscularity in adolescent boys and girls. Journal of American College Health, 48, 297–320. McGarvey, S. (1991). Obesity in Samoans and a perspective in its aetiology in Polynesians. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 53, 86–94. Miller, M. N., & Pumariega, A. J. (2001). Culture and eating disorders: A historical and cross-cultural review. Psychiatry, 64, 93–110. Mintz, L. B., & Kashubeck, S. (1999). Body image and disordered eating among Asian–American and Caucasian college students: An examination of race and gender differences. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 23, 781–796. Morry, M. M., & Staska, S. L. (2001). Magazine exposure: Internalization, self-objectification, eating attitudes, and body satisfaction in male and female university students. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Sciences, 33, 269–279. Murray, S. H., Touyz, S. W., & Beumont, P. J. V. (1996). Awareness and perceived influence of body ideals in the media: A comparison of eating disorder patients and the general community. Eating Disorders: The Journal of Treatment and Prevention, 4, 33–46. Muth, J. L., & Cash, T. F. (1997). Body-image attitudes: What difference does gender make? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 27, 1438–1452. Nasser, M. (1988). Eating disorders: The cultural dimension. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 23, 184–187. Nelson, L. D., & Morrison, E. L. (2005). The symptoms of resource scarcity: Judgements of food and finances incluence preferences for potential partners. Psychological Science, 16, 167–173. Nixon, S. (1996). Hard looks: Masculinities, spectator ship, and contemporary consumption. London: University College of London Press. Peixoto Labre, M. (2002). Adolescent boys and the muscular male body ideal. Journal of Adolescent Health, 30, 233–242. Petrie, T. A., Austin, L. J., Crowley, B. J., Helmcamp, A., Johnson, C. E., Lester, R., et al. (1996). Sociocultural expectations of attractiveness for males. Sex Roles, 35, 581–602. Powers, P. S. (1980). Obesity: The regulation of weight. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins. Ricciardelli, L. A., & McCabe, M. P. (2001a). Self-esteem and negative affect as moderators of sociocultural influences on body dissatisfaction, strategies to decrease weight, and strategies to 393 increase muscles among adolescent boys and girls. Sex Roles, 44, 189–206. Ricciardelli, L. A., & McCabe, M. P. (2001b). Dietary restraint and negative affect as mediators of body dissatisfaction and bulimic behaviour in adolescent girls and boys. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39, 1317–1328. Ross, W. D., & Ward, R. (1982). Human proportionality and sexual dimorphism. In R. L. Hall (Ed.), Sexual dimorphism in Homo sapiens: A question of size (pp. 317–361). New York: Praeger. Rudovsky, B. (1974). The unfashionable human body. New York: Anchor Books. Shackelford, T. K., Weekes-Shackelford, V. A., LeBlanc, G. J., Bleske, A. L., Euler, H. A., & Hoier, S. (2000). Female coital orgasm and male attractiveness. Human Nature: An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective, 11, 299–306. Singh, D. (1995). Female judgement of male attractiveness and desirability for relationships: Role of waist-to-hip ratio and financial status. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 1089–1101. Smith, K. L., Cornelissen, P. L., & Tovée, M. J. (2005). Judgements of physical attractiveness and health using 3D video clips of female bodies. Unpublished manuscript. Staffieri, J. R. (1967). A study of social stereotypes of body image in children. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7, 101– 104. Stam, H. (Ed.). (1998). The body and psychology. London: Sage. Swami, V., Caprario, C., Tovée, M. J., & Furnham, A. (in press). Female physical attractiveness in Britain and Japan: A crosscultural study. European Journal of Personality. Swami, V., & Tovée, M. J. (2005a). Female physical attractiveness in Britain and Malaysia: A cross-cultural study. Body Image, 2, 115–128. Swami, V., & Tovée, M. J. (2005b). Do judgements of food influence preferences for female body weight? Unpublished manuscript. Tovée, M. J., Maisey, D. S., Emery, J. L., & Cornelissen, P. L. (1999). Visual cues to female physical attractiveness. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 266, 211–218. Tukey, J. W. (1977). Exploratory data analysis. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Wetsman, A., & Marlowe, F. (1999). How universal are preferences for female waist-to-hip ratios? Evidence from the Hadza of Tanzania. Evolutio & Human Behavior, 20, 219–228. Yu, D. W., & Shepard, G. H. (1998). Is beauty in the eye of the beholder? Nature, 396, 321–322.