Investigation of a biopsychosocial perspective of pain in Brazilian chronic pain patients Jamir J. Sardá Jr. B.Sc, M.Sc. Thesis submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy The University of Sydney 2007 Supervisor’s Certificate This is to certify that the thesis entitled ”Investigation of a biopsychosocial perspective of pain in Brazilian chronic pain patients” submitted by Jamir J. Sardá Jr. in fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy is in a form ready for examination. Associate Professor Michael K. Nicholas Faculty of Medicine University of Sydney February 2007 ii Candidate’s Certificate I, Jamir J. Sardá Jr., hereby declare that the work contained within this thesis is my own and had not been submitted to any other institution as a part or a whole requirement for any higher degree. Jamir J. Sardá Jr. February 2007 iii Acknowledgments There are a number of people who collaborated directly and indirectly to the completion of this thesis, to whom I would like to acknowledge. First at all, I would like to thanks my supervisor Associate Professor Michael Nicholas, who shared his experience and knowledge with me, and was present, kind, patient, wise, and tireless in guiding me through this journey. However these words are not enough to express my gratitude to a great mentor. My co-supervisor Dr. Cibele M. A. Pimenta gave me some motivation to do this thesis a couple years before it really started, and her support during the data collection was essential. Dr. Ali Asghari was a great colleague during these 4 years. He was always keen to discuss my ideas and to motivate me. He also gave me precious statistical advises and was a true teacher. I also would like to thank several professionals who make the Pain Research and Management Institute a place of excellence and one of the best Pain Centres in the world. Professor Michael Cousins, Professor Laurie Matter, Ms. Isobel Taylor, Professor Phillip Siddall, Dr. Fiona Blyth, Dr. Stephen Loftus, Ms. Lee Beeston, Ms. Lois Tonkin, Dr. Robin Murray, Ms. Sandra Hives, Ms. Ros Wyllie and Ms. Wendy O’Hanlon. Without them the PMRC would not be what it is and I would not be able to do my thesis. iv I would like to mention a number of people in Brazil who also collaborated to this research project. Ms. Maria Luisa Lombas – Coordinator of Capes PhD Scholarship Program. Dr. Pedro Girardi – Coordinator of the course of Psychology at Univali - Itajaí, Dr. Almir Sais – Coordinator of the course of Psychology at Univali - Biguaçu, Dr. Marco Modesto, Dr. Ivan Pereira, Dr. Li Shi Mim, Dr. José Oswaldo Junior, Dr. Claúdio Corrêa, Dra. Lilian Hennemann, and all the research assistants. I would like to acknowledge all participants who were keen to give personal information about their lives aiming to contribute with the progress of science. An special thanks must go to my partner, Nelisia Medeiros, who has always give me great support, motivation, happiness and love and has been very patient over this period. Two other unique women were also present during this whole process, my mother Lurdes S.Tiago Sardá and Isobel Taylor. They are my role model of strength, courage, knowledge and resilience. If the University of Sydney permitted candidates to dedicate theses, then this thesis would be dedicated to these three women. v Abstract This series of studies is based on a biopsychosocial perspective of chronic pain. According to this perspective there is a dynamic interrelationship among biological changes, psychological status and social context playing distinct roles in chronic pain, disability and emotional adjustment. The specific aim of this thesis was to test the applicability of predictions based on a biopsychosocial model of chronic pain amongst a large sample of Brazilian chronic pain patients. The psychometric properties of a number of measures on cognitive and affective domains of chronic pain were tested. The relationships between socio-demographic, clinical and psychological factors within a Brazilian and an Australian chronic pain population were examined and compared. In this cross-sectional design study data were collected from 311 participants in each sample using questionnaires. A number of statistical analyses mainly descriptive statistics, t test, analysis of variance, multiple correlation, multiple hierarchical regression and logistic regression analyses were used to test the validity and reliability of the measures and the relationships between variables. All the analyses were conducted using the SPSS for Windows version 14.0. The results suggest that the Brazilian versions of the Roland and Morris Disability Questionnaire, the Pain Self-Efficacy Questionnaire, the DASS – Depression Scale, the PRSS – Catastrophising Scale and the Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire have generally sound psychometric properties and may be used with other Brazilian chronic pain populations. The findings vi also revealed that in these populations, some socio-demographic factors interact with clinical variables contributing to disability. Cognitions also contributed to disability, emotional adjustment, pain intensity and work status. In the Brazilian sample, educational level, pain site, and self-efficacy contributed to disability; catastrophising was the only predictor of depression; gender and self-efficacy contributed to pain severity; and age, educational level, disability and self-efficacy were risk factors for unemployment. Whilst in the Australian sample, pain severity, activity engagement and self-efficacy contributed to disability; catastrophising, selfefficacy and pain willingness were predictors of depression; self-efficacy contributed to pain severity, and age, educational level, pain site, and depression were risk factors for unemployment. These findings indicate that the Australian and the Brazilian sample presented a number of similarities and only a few differences regarding the contribution of psychosocial factors to chronic pain, disability and emotional adjustment. This confirms the hypothesis that the relationship between these factors would occur in a similar pattern in these two samples and in accordance with biopsychosocial perspectives. The results of these studies are in accordance with the pain literature generally, which reaffirms that chronic pain is a multidimensional experience mediated and moderated by similar psychosocial factors, even in different cultures. From a clinical perspective these findings suggest that interventions based on these concepts should be feasible in different populations. This series of studies is one of the first to analyse the simultaneous contribution of self-efficacy, acceptance and catastrophising to physical and vii emotional adjustment in different cultures. The findings suggest a number of lines for future research and have a number of clinical and research implications. viii Publications arising from this thesis Peer reviewed papers/book chapters Sardá, J. J Jr. Aceitação da dor crônica: novidade conceitual ou resgate de um princípio fundamental no tratamento de doenças crônicas? “Acceptance of chronic pain: conceptual novelty or returning to an old concept in the treatment of chronic illness?” In: Gildo Angelotti (Org). Terapia Cognitivo-Comportamental no Tratamento da Dor. São Paulo: Casa do Psicólogo. (in press). Sardá, J. J Jr.; Nicholas, M. K.; Pimenta, C. A. M.; Asghari, A. Pain related self-efficacy beliefs in a Brazilian chronic pain patient sample: A psychometric analysis. Stress and Health. (in press). Sardá, J. J Jr.; Nicholas, M. K.; Pimenta, C. A. M.; Asghari, A. Psychometric properties of the DASS- Depression scale among a Brazilian population with chronic pain. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. (in press). Sardá, J. J. Jr.; Angelotti, G. Avaliação Psicológica da Dor. “Psychological Assessment of Pain” In: Joao Augusto Bertuol Figueiro; Gildo Angelotti, Cibele A. Mattos Pimenta. (Org.). Dor e Saúde Mental. São Paulo: Atheneu, 2004, p. 51-65. Conference and seminar presentations Sardá, J. J. Jr.; Nicholas, M. K.; Pimenta, C. A. M.; Asghari, A.; Corrêa, C. F.; Oswaldo, J. Jr.; Hennmann, L.; Pereira, I.; Min, L. S. Validade e fidedignidade do questionario Roland Morris de Incapacidade em uma população de brasileiros com dor crônica. “Validity and reliability of the RMDQ in a Brazilian population with chronic pain” In: 7º Congresso Brasileiro de Dor, 2006, Gramado. ix Sardá, J. J. Jr.; Nicholas, M. K.; Pimenta, C. A. M.; Asghari, A. Psychological Assessment of Chronic Pain Patients: Available Measures in Brazil. In: 11th World Congress on Pain, 2005, Sydney. Abstracts. Seattle: IASP, 2005. v. 1. p.325. Sardá, J. J. Jr. Perspectivas clínicas e em pesquisa no tratamento psicológico de pacientes com dor. “ Research and clinical perspectives in the treatment of chronic pain patients” In: VII Simposio Brasileiro e Encontro Internacional sobre Dor, 2005, São Paulo. VII Simbidor, 2005. v. 1. p. 372-77. x Table of Contents Page Supervisor’s Certificate ...................................................................... ii Candidate’s Certificate ........................................................................ iii Acknowledgments .............................................................................. iv Abstract ........................................................................................... vi Publications arising from this thesis ..................................................... ix Table of contents .............................................................................. xi List of figures .................................................................................... xvi List of tables ..................................................................................... xvi Chapter 1: Introduction ................................................................. 1 1.1 Definition of Pain ......................................................................... 1 1.2 Epidemiology of Chronic Pain ................................................... 3 1.2.1 Summary of Findings ........................................................... 8 1.3 Pain Theories and Models .............................................................. 10 1.4. Disability .................................................................................... 18 1.4.1 Disability Measures .............................................................. 20 1.4.2 Comments .......................................................................... 25 Chapter 2: Psychosocial Factors Involved in Chronic Pain .............. 27 2.1 Cognitive Factors ........................................................................ 33 2.1.1 Coping Strategies ................................................................ 34 2.1.1.1 The Contribution of Coping Strategies to Adjustment to Chronic Pain ................................................................. 37 2.1.2 Beliefs ................................................................................ 41 2.1.2.1 Self-efficacy Beliefs ........................................................... 45 2.1.2.1.1 The Contribution of Self-efficacy to Adjustment to Chronic Pain .................................................................. 47 xi 2.1.2.1.2 Measures of Pain Self-efficacy Beliefs ................................ 49 2.1.2.1.3 Comments .................................................................... 52 2.1.2.2 Catastrophising ................................................................ 55 2.1.2.2.1 The Contribution of Catastrophising to Adjustment to Chronic Pain .................................................................. 56 2.1.2.2.2 Catastrophising Measures ................................................ 60 2.1.2.2.3 Comments .................................................................... 64 2.2 Attitudes ..................................................................................... 68 2.2.1 Acceptance of Chronic Pain .................................................. 68 2.2.1.1 The contribution of Acceptance to Adjustment to Chronic Pain .................................................................. 70 2.2.1.2 Acceptance Measures ....................................................... 75 2.2.1.3 Comments ...................................................................... 77 2.3 Affective Factors ......................................................................... 81 2.3.1 The Contribution of Depression to Disability Associated with Chronic Pain ..................................................................... 81 2.3.2 Depression Measurement ................................................. 86 2.3.3 Comments ...................................................................... 91 2.4 Cultural Factors and Pain............................................................... 93 2.4.1 Contribution of Cultural Factors to Chronic Pain ...................... 93 2.4.2. Comments ...................................................................... 103 2.5 Psychological Assessment ........................................................... 105 2.5.1 Psychological Assessment Indications and Aims ................... 105 2.5.2 Psychological Assessment Methods ...................................... 107 2.6 Final comments .......................................................................... 112 Chapter 3: Study One: Psychometric Properties of Measures of Disability, Pain Self-Efficacy Beliefs, Catastrophising, Acceptance and Depression in a Brazilian Population with Chronic Pain 3.1 Introduction ............................................................................. 113 3.2 Method ..................................................................................... 117 3.2.1 Subjects ........................................................................... 117 3.2.2 Inclusion criteria ............................................................... 117 xii 3.2.3 Exclusion criteria ............................................................... 118 3.2.4 Procedure ......................................................................... 118 3.2.5 Measures ......................................................................... 119 3.3 Aims of study .............................................................................. 122 3.4 Statistical analyses ....................................................................... 122 3.5 Results ................................................................................ 123 3.6 Discussion ................................................................................... 146 3.6.1 Psychometric properties of the RMDQ Brazilian version ........... 146 3.6.2 Psychometric properties of the PRSS – Catastrophising Scale Brazilian version ........................................................ 149 3.6.3 Psychometric properties of the DASS – Depression Scale Brazilian version ................................................................ 152 3.6.4 Psychometric properties of the Pain Self-Efficacy Questionnaire PSEQ Brazilian version .............................................................. 155 3.6.5 Psychometric properties of the Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire - CPAQ Brazilian version .................. 159 3.6.6 Summary of discussion ....................................................... 165 Chapter 4: Study Two: The relative contributions of self-efficacy beliefs, catastrophising and acceptance to disability and depression in a Brazilian population with chronic pain 4.1 Introduction ................................................................................ 167 4.2 Method ...................................................................................... 170 4.2.1 Subjects ............................................................................ 170 4.2.2 Inclusion and exclusion criteria ............................................. 170 4.2.3 Procedure .......................................................................... 170 4.2.4 Measures ........................................................................... 170 4.3 Hypothesis ................................................................................. 171 4.4 Aims of study .............................................................................. 171 4.5 Statistical analyses ....................................................................... 172 4.6 Results ....................................................................................... 173 4.6.1 Summary of findings ........................................................... 187 4.7 Discussion ................................................................................... 189 xiii 4.7.1 Contribution of socio-demographic, clinical and psychological variables to disability ................................................................ 189 4.7.1.1 Contribution of socio-demographic and clinical variables to disability ............................................................................. 190 4.7.1.2 Contribution of psychological variables to disability .............. 191 4.7.2.1 Contribution of socio-demographic, clinical and psychological variables to depression ............................................................ 196 4.7.2.2 Contribution of psychological variables to depression ........... 199 4.7.3 Contribution of socio-demographic, clinical and psychological variables to pain intensity ........................................................ 204 4.7.3.1 Contribution of socio-demographic and clinical variables to pain intensity ...................................................................... 204 4.7.3.2 Contribution of psychological variables to pain intensity ........ 205 4.7.4 Examining risk factors for work status .................................. 208 4.7.5 Summary of discussion and implications ................................ 215 Chapter 5: Study Three: A cross-cultural comparison of the contribution of cognitions to disability and depression: A comparison between an Australian and a Brazilian sample with chronic pain. 5.1 Introduction ................................................................................ 217 5.2 Socio-demographic characteristics of Brazil and Australia ................. 219 5.2.1 Brazil ................................................................................ 219 5.2.2 Australia ........................................................................... 221 5.3 Method ...................................................................................... 225 5.3.1 Subjects ............................................................................ 225 5.3.2 Inclusion and exclusion criteria ............................................. 225 5.3.3 Procedure .......................................................................... 225 5.3.4 Measures ........................................................................... 226 5.4 Hypothesis ................................................................................. 226 5.5 Aims of study .............................................................................. 227 5.6 Statistical analyses ....................................................................... 227 5.7 Results ..................................................................................... 228 5.8 Discussion ................................................................................... 244 xiv 5.8.1 Variables contributing to disability in the Australian and in the Brazilian sample ....................................................... 252 5.8.2 Variables contributing to depression in the Australian and in the Brazilian sample ....................................................... 255 5.8.3 Variables contributing to pain intensity in the Australian and in the Brazilian sample ....................................................... 256 5.8.4 Examining risk factors for work status in the Australian and in the Brazilian sample ...................................................... 257 5.8.5 Summary of discussion ...................................................... 259 Chapter 6: General Discussion 6.1 General discussion ....................................................................... 260 6.2 Limitations and strengthens........................................................... 273 6.3 Conclusion................................................................................... 276 6.4 Further directions ......................................................................... 278 References ..................................................................................... 280 Appendices Appendix I. List of institutions participating in the study ........................ 309 Appendix II. The consent form............................................................. 311 Appendix III. Ethic committee approvals............................................... 313 Appendix IV. Original measures and translations ................................... 323 Appendix V. Publications .................................................................... 338 xv List of figures Figure 1. Biopsychosocial Model of Pain ................................................ 13 Figure 2. Psychobiological Model of Chronic Pain .................................... 16 Figure 3. Overview of Problems Associated with Chronic Pain .................. 29 Figure 4. A Multistage model of cognition, disability and affect ............... 85 List of tables Study One Table 3.1 Socio-demographic characteristics of the study sample .......... 124 Table 3.2 Clinical characteristics of the study sample ........................... 126 Table 3.3 Descriptive statistics for all the measures ............................. 129 Table 3.4 Comparison between mean scores of the Brazilian sample and the original questionnaire............................................. 130 Table 3.5 Internal consistency and split-half correlation of the measures . 131 Table 3.6 Item-scale correlation for the CPAQ ..................................... 133 Table 3.7 PCA of the CPAQ factors loading and communalities for a 5 factor solution ......................................................... 135 Table 3.8 Means (SD) and internal consistency for a 4 factor solution...... 136 Table 3.9 PCA of the PSEQ factors loading and communalities for a 1 factor solution ............................................................. 137 Table 3.10 PCA of the Catastrophising Scale factors loading and communalities for a 2 factor solution .............................. 138 Table 3.11 Mean (SD) and internal consistency for a 2 factor solution ..... 139 Table 3.12 PCA of the Depression Scale with factors loading and communalities for a 1 factor solution ............................. 140 Table 3.13 Comparison of mean score in questionnaires by educational level .......................................................... 141 Tables 3.14 Comparison of mean scores on questionnaires by working status .............................................................. 142 Table 3.15 Correlations between questionnaires, scales and CPAQ factors 144 xvi Study Two Table 4.1 Pearson correlations among socio-demographic, clinical and psychological measures ................................................ 175 Table 4.2 Multiple hierarchical regression analysis predicting disability .... 178 Table 4.3 Multiple hierarchical regression analysis predicting depression .. 180 Table 4.3.a MHRA predicting depression from catastrophising factors ...... 182 Table 4.4 Multiple hierarchical regression analysis predicting pain intensity ................................................................... 183 Table 4.5 Logistic regression analysis for work status (Brazilian sample) . 186 Study Three Table 5.1 Socio-demographic and clinical characteristics of the Australian sample ............................................................... 229 Table 5.2 Descriptive statistics for the Australian sample in all the measures ............................................................... 231 Table 5.3 Comparison of scores on questionnaires by educational level ... 232 Table 5.3 Comparison of scores on questionnaires by educational level ... 233 Table 5.5 Pearson correlations among socio-demographic, clinical and psychological measures ................................................ 234 Table 5.6 MHRA predicting disability (Australian sample) ....................... 237 Table 5.7 MHRA predicting depression (Australian sample) .................... 238 Table 5.8 MHRA predicting pain intensity (Australian sample) ................. 239 Table 5.9 Logistic regression analysis for work status (Australian sample) 242 Table 5.10 Socio-demographic and clinical characteristics of the Australian and Brazilian sample .......................................................... 244 Table 5.11 Comparison between questionnaire means of the Brazilian and Australian samples...................................................... 247 xvii 1. Introduction The main purpose of this literature review is to: (I) describe the biopsychosocial model of chronic pain, and (II) to explore some key cognitive and affective aspects of pain. This review also provides an assessment of a number of commonly used psychological measures, which will provide a framework for the development of a battery of measures to assess different aspects of pain in a Brazilian and an Australian chronic pain population. 1.1 Definition of Pain According to the International Association for the Study of Pain (IASP) definition, “pain is an unpleasant sensory and emotional experience associated with actual or potential tissue damage, or described in terms of such damage” (Merskey and Bogduk 1994). Although this definition is not totally satisfactory, there is a general consensus that pain is a multidimensional phenomenon and its relationship to tissue damage and suffering is variable. There are several pain taxonomies, usually based on area or site, as well as mechanism and duration. Time - based classification of pain includes acute, acute recurrent, chronic, chronic progressive and laboratory-induced acute pain. According to Turk and Melzack (2001), acute pain is typically referred to as pain with a relatively short duration (hours, days or a few weeks, up to a maximum of 4 weeks). Pain that persists from 4 to 12 weeks is often described as sub-acute pain. Acute recurrent pain is characterised by the presence of acute pain interspersed with periods without any pain. Pain that persists for a period longer than 3 months is described as chronic pain. Chronic progressive pain is characterised by the evolution of pain intensity associated with a progressive disease (e.g. Cancer). Laboratory-induced acute pain consists of experimental induced pain (e.g. cold pressor task). The IASP Subcommittee on Taxonomy established three time categories for pain: less than one month, one to six months and over six months. Although, there are a few differences in pain definition, for nonmalignant pain three months is the most convenient point of division between acute and chronic pain (Merskey and Bogduk 1994). This literature review will adopt the classification for chronic pain, as pain that persists for a period longer than three months. This definition is consistent with commonly-used epidemiology studies (Blyth et al. 2001; Eriksen et al. 2003; Von Korff 1999). Pain is the most common symptom and the leading reason given for seeking health care (Gureje et al., 1998; Turk and Melzack, 2001). In most acute disease it is an important warning signal and leads to a diagnosis and subsequent treatment. However, in a significant number of people acute pain evolves to chronic pain, which in most cases has no cure or time limit. Recently, some have argued that it may be thought of as disease entity in itself (Siddall and Cousins 2004). To illustrate the magnitude of this problem epidemiological data will be presented. 2 1.2 Epidemiology of Chronic Pain The prevalence of chronic pain has been found to vary among different populations. In part, this is due to differences in definition and research methods. Using a definition of chronic pain as pain that has lasted longer than three months, Von Korff et al. (1990) reported a prevalence of 45% of persisting or recurrent pain in a sample of 1,016 adults randomly select from a health service in Washington State. In this sample 8% of the participants reported severe and persistent pain, whilst in 3.7% pain was associated with disability. A study conducted in New Zealand (James et al. 1991) in a community sample with 1,498 participants found that over 80% of the adult population is affected by pain at some stage of their life. However their definition of pain did not specify the criteria used to establish the duration of the pain, nor severity and impact. Their results also suggest heterogeneity of pain complaints, and the association of some pain syndromes and age. A survey conducted by Gureje et al. (1998) with 25,916 participants in fifteen countries in Asia, Africa, Americas and Europe, used the definition of persistent pain as pain that is present most of the time for a period of six months or more during the prior year. This group found a persistent pain prevalence of 22% among patients using the primary care system, with some variation among centres. In this study, participants suffering from persistent pain were more likely to be female, to have an anxiety or depression disorder, and to experience significant 3 activity limitation. In a review of published studies Von Korff et al. (1999) described a prevalence of chronic pain in general populations ranging from 7% to 40%. However, these authors emphasise that this wide range is mainly due to the use of different pain definitions. Blyth et al. (2001) reviewed fifteen studies and found a pain prevalence of 15%, ranging from 2% to 40%. A review conducted by Harstall and Ospina (2003) with thirteen studies published between 1991 and 2002 found a chronic pain prevalence range from 10.1% to 55.2%, with a higher pain prevalence among females and a higher incidence of musculoskeletal pain. Among these studies six used the IASP chronic pain definition (3 months), and four used the definition of persistent pain (pain lasting longer than 6 months). Three studies adopted the American College of Rheumatology (ACP) definition of chronic widespread pain, which is similar to the IASP definition but includes the presence of pain in several different areas. A recent survey conducted in 15 countries in Europe and Israel with general population (n=49,394 participants) have found a 19% prevalence of chronic pain, affecting quality of social and working life (Breivik et al. 2006). Using the IASP definition of persisting pain (pain that lasts for at least 3 months in the last 6 months), in an Australian community sample of 17,000 selected by a random-digit dialling, Blyth et al., (2001) reported a chronic pain prevalence of 17.1% among males and 20% among females. In this study chronic pain was associated with aging, female gender, lower socioeconomic level and psychological distress. In 4 Denmark (Eriksen et al. 2003) found a similar chronic pain prevalence, 16% for men and 21% for women also associated with aging, lower education and economic levels. Similar results were found in Spain in a general population survey, with a 23.4% chronic pain prevalence, also associated with aging and daily activities interference due to pain (Catala et al. 2002). A 10.8% prevalence of chronic pain was reported in a sample of 1.000 of Hong Kong’s Chinese adult population (Ng et al. 2002). A study conducted in Canada with a stratified random sample (n=2,012), using a definition of chronic pain as continuous or intermittent pain present for at least six months, reported a prevalence of 29% of non-malignant chronic pain (Moulin et al. 2002). Other studies with specific chronic pain syndromes or specific groups (eg. elderly, children and indigenous population) have also reported a high prevalence. A prospective longitudinal study in France reported a prevalence of 7.8% of chronic neck and shoulder pain in men and 14.8% among women, with a close association between psychosocial factors and chronic pain (Cassou et al. 2002). This study investigated only the working-age population and defined chronic pain as pain that lasted longer than six months and considered some specific sites only. A review on chronic back pain in adults in several countries described a prevalence between 15-30% (Andersson 1999). A survey conducted in Germany by Kohlmann (2003) found a prevalence of 16% of musculoskeletal pain among the adult population. According to Neumann and Buskila (2003), the prevalence of fibromyalgia in the general population ranges from 0.5% to 5%. Two English surveys 5 reported variations in prevalence according to pain site. The first study, (Aggarwal et al. 2003) with 2,504 subjects, found a 26% prevalence of oro-facial pain, with a higher prevalence rate among those people in lower socio-economic levels. The second survey, (Webb et al. 2003) had a sample of 5,752 adults and found a prevalence of 14% of neck and back chronic pain. Another cross-sectional study in the Dutch general population found a prevalence of 26.9% for back pain, 20.9% for shoulder pain and 20.6% for neck pain (Picavet and Schouten 2003). Honeyman and Jacobs (1996) in a study with an aboriginal population in Central Australia found a pain prevalence range from 30% to 50%. They reported a higher pain tolerance among the participants and no illness or pain behaviours that are commonly described in North American and European studies were observed (e.g. limping, guarding and facial expressions). Pain duration was difficult to evaluate due to cultural aspects and language factors, but low-back pain was the most common chronic pain reported, followed by neck pain. Most pain prevalence studies have been done in United States, Canada, Europe, Australia and New Zealand. It has been thought that some types of pain would be associated with their Western society lifestyle (e.g. low-back pain) (Andersson 1999; Honeyman and Jacobs 1996), however recent evidence has not supported this hypothesis. Epidemiological data on pain prevalence from developing countries are scarce. Sharma et al. (2003) reported a 23.09% pain prevalence in a rural population of inpatients and outpatients that were referred to a rehabilitation institution in India. Wang (2003) reviewed several studies 6 in Asia and found a prevalence of tension-type headache between 8.4% and 12.7%. This result is in the lower range of headache prevalence reported by International Headache Society (HIS) in Western countries (15.6% to 25.7%). Volinn (1997) in an epidemiological study found an 18.5% low back pain prevalence rate in lower income countries (e.g. India, Brazil, South Africa). In Brazil, Pimenta (2001) conducted a study with a community randomly selected sample (n=1,871) and found a 28.7% chronic pain prevalence among children, 61.4% among adults, and in an elderly group the prevalence was 51.4%. Overall the relationship between chronic pain and gender, age, social class and level of education was not clear. Children and adults complained of daily interference in their activities due to pain. Among adults headaches were more frequent in women. Younger women reported a higher incidence of pelvic-pain, and low-back pain was associated with lower socio-economic classes. Data from a survey conducted by Gureje et al. (1998) with a stratified random sample in Brazil, reported a persistent pain prevalence of 31% among participants using the primary health system. Another study conducted by Sardá et al. (2003) with a convenience sample of 182 university students, found a chronic pain prevalence of 53%. Low back pain and headache were the most common symptoms and levels of anxiety and depression were higher than in the general population. The results showed a higher prevalence of chronic pain when compared to findings reported in the international literature; however this study had several methodological 7 limitations and was not intended to be representative of the general population. 1.2.1 Summary of Findings The prevalence of chronic pain varies significantly among different populations (i.e. countries and subgroups) from 10% to 50%, but a mean prevalence of 15% is a well-supported estimation. The different findings are likely to be related to variations in methodologies and definitions. It is notable that most of these studies were conducted mainly in working-age populations, thus there is still lack of information regarding chronic pain in elderly, younger and specific populations (e.g. aborigines). Several studies have reported an association between psychological factors, demographic and economic variables and the prevalence of chronic pain, suggesting that the experience and impact of chronic pain involve many factors. Among socio-demographic variables, gender (i.e. female) and lower social classes have been associated with a higher prevalence of chronic pain. Regarding psychological factors, most studies have found an association between variables such as depression and anxiety, and higher levels of disability or unemployment. Chronic pain has a high direct and indirect cost to the health system as well as an economic impact for the whole community (i.e. missing working days, insurance costs, costs of treatment) and psychosocial impact (i.e. disability and emotional comorbidities), not to 8 mention the likely effects on the families of those with chronic pain (Blyth et al. 2005; Turk 2002a). There is evidence suggesting that chronic pain is a major health problem in most societies. However, it is important to recognise that current theoretical models of pain and most epidemiological findings have emerged from higher income western societies. Whether these models and epidemiological findings are applicable in developing (or low income) countries and other cultures are largely unknown. Given that psychological, functional and environmental interactions seem important in understanding the impact of chronic pain, and taking into account that psychosocial factors may be responsive to interventions, it would seem worthwhile to investigate the applicability of current models of chronic pain in the Brazilian population. Brazil represents a country in transition from lower to higher income with wide disparities. The common language spoken is Portuguese. This investigation will require the development or adaptation of English (language) psychological measures to assess the range of dimensions covered by these models. If the relationship between psychological variables and pain in the Brazilian chronic pain population is found to be consistent with evidence from the more developed countries, it would suggest that treatments found to be effective in these other countries might also be applicable in Brazil. In the next section a number of relevant pain theoretical models will be described. 9 1.3 Pain Theories and Models In this literature review a number of theoretical models about chronic pain will be briefly described, but the main focus will be on the biopsychosocial perspectives. Current pain theories have evolved from earlier models (e.g. Sensory and Pattern theories) to the Gate Control Theory (Melzack and Wall 1965) and to the Neuromatrix model (Loeser and Melzack 1999). Some earlier models focused more on sensory components of pain and viewed psychological aspects only as responses to pain. These theoretical models tried to explain pain in terms of a stimulus-response process. Broadly these uni-dimensional models understood pain as a specific sensation directly related to specific pathways and the amount of nociceptive input (Flor et al. 1990). Although earlier sensory models contributed to the understanding of pain phenomena, cumulative evidence and clinical observations (e.g. phantom limb pain) collaborated to diminish the uni-dimensional pain theories and forced the development of other theoretical models. The later models attempt to address the limitations of the previous models, combining new evidence of neurophysiological mechanisms with psychological processes. The Gate Control Theory (GCT) (Melzack and Wall 1965) provided a theoretical basis for expanding the comprehension of pain beyond merely the noxious physiopathologic stimuli, components the of sensorial pain. This dimension model and the suggested the 10 existence of ascending and descending processes modulated by the brain and other elements of the central nervous system (Loeser and Melzack 1999). The GCT emphasised the role of the brain and the dorsal horn of the spinal cord as active systems participating in the modulation of noxious stimuli (Melzack 1996). Probably the major contributions of the GCT have been to highlight the key role of the central nervous system as an active component in pain processes and response with excitatory and inhibitory roles, and the participation of affective and cognitive components in the pain phenomenon (Turk and Monarch, 2002). On the other hand, one of the limitations of the GCT was that it did not explain how psychological processes contributed to modulate pain. It is also important to realise that the GCT intended to provide an account for acute pain where there is a noxious input from the periphery, rather than chronic pain where no peripheral input may be involved (Nicholas and Molloy 2002; Turk and Monarch 2002). Although subsequently neurophysiological research has revealed far more about the working of the peripheral and central nervous systems than was known in 1965 the GCT is regarded as the foundation of the modern concepts of pain (Zimmermann 2005). More recently the Neuromatrix model was developed aiming to address the role of a range of factors on the modulation of inputs through brain patterns. Loeser and Melzack (1999) proposed that afferent inputs act on this neuromatrix and produce output patterns that lead to the report of pain. This pattern generating mechanism (Neuromatrix) is 11 thought to be influenced by cultural factors, past experience and personality variables, attentional processes, medullary descending inhibition, autonomic, endocrine and immune changes, central-nervous system plasticity, pathogenic inputs, viscerosensory and somatosensory inputs. The Neuromatrix model hypothesises that the output of the neuromatrix, not the input, is able to generate neurosignature patterns for pain and even act in the absence of inputs. This may explain some types of pain that are characterised by the absence of a discernible stimulus or a lack of relation between pathology and pain intensity (Melzack, 1996; Loeser and Melzack, 1999). This model proposes a range of possibilities for pain modulation, but at a more central level. This model proposes that nociceptive stimulation may produce neurological structural and functional changes, which explain why some individuals experience a gradual pain increase after an initial sensitisation. A number of studies have provided support for the Neuromatrix model describing neuroplasticity and sensitisation properties of the peripheral and nervous system (Siddall and Cousins 1998; Siddall and Cousins 2004). These two processes have a central role in explaining physiological and psychological changes in chronic pain. The GCT and Neuromatrix theories have provided a framework for understanding the role of psychological and physiological factors in pain. However, both models have limitations regarding how cognitive and 12 emotional factors might mediate/moderate physiological changes associated with chronic pain. Parallel to the development of these primarily neurophysiological pain theories other researchers have worked on concepts of illness and factors involved biopsychosocial in it model (e.g. Engel proposed by 1977, Mechanic, 1962). The Engel (1977) suggested that symptoms should be conceptualised as a result of dynamic interactions between psychological, social and pathophysiological variables. Engels’ model was an attempt to overcome the reductionism and limitations of the traditional biomedical approach, which is centred on pathophysiological factors only. A number of researchers (Flor et al. 1990; Loeser 1980; Turk et al. 1983; Waddell 1998) have adapted the original biopsychosocial model to chronic pain. Social Environment Illness Behaviour Suffering Pain Perception Nociception Figure 1. Biopsychosocial Model of Pain (Waddell 1998). 13 The premise of this biopsychosocial model is that nociceptive components are one contributor to pain experience, interacting continuously with psychological and social factors. Based on this assumption, it is proposed that nociception will influence pain perception, suffering, illness and pain behaviour but these domains will be mediated and moderated1 by psychological and environmental factors. These relationships are not uni-directional but multi-determinate and dynamic. Although Waddell (1998) did not intend to describe all the factors participating in chronic pain, his model illustrates the overlap and the dynamic of variables participating in the experience of pain. The biopsychosocial perspectives propose the existence of a dynamic interrelationship among biological changes, psychological processes and social context playing distinct roles in pain. According to this perspective “Biological factors may initiate, maintain, and modulate physical perturbations; psychological factors influence the appraisal and perception of internal physiological signs, and social factors shape the behavioural response of patients to the perception of their physical perturbations” (Turk, 1996, p.6). Although conceptually important the early biopsychosocial models of pain lacked evidence on how psychosocial factors interact with biological aspects. However evidence regarding these interactions has built up during the last 20 years. 1 Although the term mediator and moderator are often used interchangeably these terms are distinct and should be clarified to avoid further misunderstandings. A moderator variable affects the relationship between two variables in a way that the impact of the independent variable on the dependent variable varies according to the value of the moderator (i.e. pain intensity - educational level - disability). On the other hand, with a mediator variable the relationship between a independent variable and a dependent variable is influenced through the mediator variable (i.e. pain intensity – self-efficacy – disability) (Holmbeck 1997). 14 It has been found that psychological factors can promote changes in the endocrine and the autonomic nervous system which may predispose the onset of a disease (Bandura et al. 1987; Flor et al. 1985). Other findings suggest that a number of behavioural aspects (i.e. pain behaviours) may reduce physical activity and consequently muscle flexibility, muscle tone, strength and physical endurance, contributing to physical deconditioning (Flor et al. 1990). Bandura’s (1985) findings also indicated that cognitive factors may have a direct effect on physiological parameters, associated more directly with the production or exacerbation of nociception. Cognitive interpretation and affective arousal may directly affect physiology by increasing sympathetic nervous system arousal, production of endogenous opioids (i.e. endorphins) (Bandura et al., 1987), and elevating the levels of muscle tension (Flor et al. 1992). Research conducted by Turk (1996) and Edwards et al. (2001a; 2001b) has shown that race and ethnicity may also influence pain perception and response to it. Their findings support the biopsychosocial perspective and help to explain how pain experience is shaped by interactions among biological, psychological and social variables. Other researchers (e.g. Bates, 1987) have included cultural and social components of the pain experience in their biopsychosocial model. Bates proposed that "there is evidence that social learning is instrumental in the development of meanings for and attitudes toward pain. Learned values and attitudes affect one's attention to painful stimuli and one's memories of prior pain experiences… Therefore, it is likely that cultural group experiences influence the physiological processes responsible for 15 pain threshold and perception of pain severity, as well as pain response" (Bates, 1987, p. 48). This biocultural perspective of pain suggests that social comparison and social learning processes within ethnic-cultural group situations influence attitudes toward pain, attention to pain stimuli or sensation and thus cognitive control. Prior pain experiences also participate in this process, moderating the descending inhibitory control (Bates 1987; Edwards et al. 2001a) Recent formulations of the biopsychosocial model of chronic pain (Flor and Hermann 2004) have described in more detail, and provided evidence about, how the factors presented in earlier biopsychosocial models might work in terms of mechanisms. Eliciting stimuli • Predisposing factors • Genetic determination • Learning • Occupational factors Aversive external / internal stimuli Psychophysiological response stereotypy e.g. symptom specific EMG increase s Pain responses Maintaining processes • Subjective-verbal • Behavioural-motor • Physiological-organic • Operant conditioning • Responden t conditioning Eliciting responses • Lack of coping skills • Inadequate perception and interpretation of physiological processes and bodily symptoms • Anticipation of pain • Memory of pain • Lack of self-efficacy Figure 2. Psychobiological model of chronic pain (Flor and Hermann, 2004). 16 Broadly this model outlines that external or internal eliciting stimuli could trigger psychophysiological responses, which are influenced by predispositional factors and eliciting responses. These psychophysiological responses will lead to pain responses, which will influence and be influenced by maintaining processes. Maintaining processes also influence eliciting responses. The biopsychosocial perspective of chronic pain has made a number of contributions to the comprehension of pain phenomena. First, these models have provided a way of integrating evidence of physiological changes and psychosocial processes and the dynamic interaction of these two aspects. Second, these perspectives have provided an alternative to more biological and reductionist explanations. Third, these models also provided the basis for interventions that may address multiple targets rather than just nociceptive processes. In the next section, the concept of disability in chronic pain will be explored. 17 1.4. Disability Chronic pain can have an enormous impact on ability to function. However, this is not universal. While some people become severely or moderately disabled, others seem to adjust reasonably well to chronic pain. Because chronic pain is not synonymous with disability, and there is a low association between impairment and disability, it is important to identify other factors that promote adaptive physical and psychological functioning (Jensen et al. 1992; Turk and Monarch 2002). The term disability has had varying definitions. The conceptualisation of disability based on a biopsychosocial model is adopted by the World Health Organisation (World Health Organization 2002), and includes the role of health conditions (disease, disorders and injuries) and external factors (environmental and personal factors) in shaping disability. According to the International Classification of Functioning - ICF (World Health Organization 2002), disability is defined as the lack (resulting from an impairment) of ability to perform an activity in the manner or within the range considered normal for a human being. To better understand this concept, two other major concepts are central to understand disability; these are impairment and handicap. Impairment is conceptualised as problems in body function or structure such as a significant deviation or loss. Handicap is defined as an individual disadvantage resulting from impairment or disabilities that limit a person’s normal roles. 18 Although the WHO’s definition of disability is well accepted, and the ICF 2002 version emphasises more the role of environmental and personal factors in disability than the 1980 document, it still relies on a scheme of causal relations, with physical damage or impairment, leading to disability and handicap. Main et al. (2005) have described different types of disability model. Some focus more on biological features, others on occupational aspects, or societal perspectives. In the chronic pain context as well as other chronic diseases, disability seems to be intrinsically related to psychosocial factors rather than only with the biological (Waddell 2003). Robinson (2001) defines disability as an inability to carry out tasks in any important domain of life due to a medical condition; including mental disorders, such as depression. Robinson’s use of the term disability implicates inability, incapacity or restricted function not only those caused by somatic factors. Pain and disability may be related to each other, but it is clear there is no one to one relationship between them. Frequently clinical improvements and decrease in pain may lead to small or unnoticeable changes in disability and quality of life (Kovacs et al. 2004). Although pain is influenced by biological factors and there is evidence of a relationship between pain intensity and disability, disability seems to be more related to and mediated by psychosocial factors (Asghari and Nicholas 2001; Kovacs et al. 2004; Main et al. 2005). Based on these perspectives, this thesis will be guided by the WHO (2002) concept, which defines disability as the lack (resulting from 19 impairment) of ability to perform an activity in the manner or within the range considered normal for a human being. However it will be complemented by Robinson’s (2001) definition of disability, which takes into account that an inability to carry out tasks due to an impairment could be occur due to a medical or psychological condition. This view is in accordance with recent evidence described above, which examined the role of psychosocial factors in disability. Further evidence will be described during this thesis. Altogether there is evidence that a number of psychosocial factors may contribute to disability. Thus, psychological assessment of the factors involved in disability is an essential issue in treatment and research. In the next section a number of disability measures will be described. 1.4.1 Disability Measures Waddell (1998) pointed out that clinical assessment of disability usually depends on the patient’s own report. Frequently, clinical interview and a clinical functional capacity evaluation provide a reliable assessment of daily activities disability, however these assessment methods are often complemented with self-reports. Disability questionnaires can be useful tools. They are more consistent and reliable than interviews, due to the invariability of the presenting questions and they are also a quick and efficient way to collect 20 information. Data provided by questionnaires also can be compared with other data, providing a more precise analysis. Furthermore, they are essential to research. On the other hand, reliability, validity and responsiveness of disability questionnaires must be demonstrated. Among several measures, some of the most frequently used disability questionnaires in the pain field are the Sickness Impact Profile, the Roland and Morris Disability Questionnaire, the Oswestry Low-Back Pain Disability Questionnaire, the Medical Outcome Study 36 Short-Form Health Survey (Battié and May 2001; Robinson 2001). The Sickness Impact Profile – SIP (Bergner et al. 1981) was one of the first self-report measures developed to assess the impact of health problems in physical and psychological disability. It is a behavioural based measure of health status, has 136 items and assesses 12 areas of functioning (functional problems in ambulation, mobility, body care and movement, social interaction, communication, alertness emotional behaviour as well as problems in sleeping, eating, working, home management and recreation), yielding 3 scales. The SIP is a reliable and valid measure, with reasonable responsiveness properties, but some items do not refer to pain problems, which may decrease its validity, reliability and sensitivity when compared to other chronic pain disability measures (Jensen et al. 1992; McDowell and Newell 1996). Another main disadvantage of this questionnaire is its length and complex scoring procedure; it takes 20 to 30 minutes to administer. These concerns led to the development of other measures (e.g. RMDQ). 21 The Roland and Morris Disability Questionnaire - RMDQ (Roland and Morris 1983) was initially developed with items from the SIP to measure self-rated physical disability in back pain patients. It has 24 items scored 0 or 1 (yes and no) and the total score varies from 0 (suggesting no disability) to 24 (severe disability). The RMDQ is a simple measure, usually takes 5 minutes and is easy to calculate and analyse. Several studies have described the strength of the RMDQ’s psychometric properties compared with other measures, specially its validity, reliability and responsiveness as a measure of physical dysfunction in chronic pain patients (Lurie 2000; Robinson 2001; Roland and Fairbank 2000; Stroud et al. 2004). RMDQ correlates well with other disability measures in physical function, such as the SF 36, SIP and Oswestry Questionnaire (Donald et al. 1995; Jensen et al. 1992; Turner et al. 2003). The RMDQ has been validated in 12 languages and adapted to other chronic pain populations with good results (Roland and Fairbank 2000). A study conducted by Nusbain et al. (2001) described satisfactory reliability and validity of a Brazilian translation of this measure. A study conducted by Donald et al. (1995) with a modified version of the RMDQ for sciatica patients showed nearly equivalent effect sizes and responsiveness when compared with the SF-36. Asghari and Nicholas (2001) also reported adequate psychometric properties for the RMDQ for chronic pain patients in general in an Australian sample. In this study these authors changed the term “back” to “pain” in all items, and item 13 was changed to “I am in pain almost all the time”. Jensen et al. (1992) 22 have reported the same adaptation with good results in a North American sample. When compared with the Oswestry Disability Questionnaire, the RMDQ seems to be more sensitive to detect changes in disability in patients with minor or moderate disability, while the Oswestry Disability Questionnaire appears to be more suitable to patients with severe disabilities. Turner et al. (2003) reported that in patients involved in worker’s compensation the RMDQ showed much more responsiveness to change and a greater ability to discriminate patients who were working from those who were not than the SF-12 and SF-36. Similar findings have been reported by Grotle et al. (2005). The Oswestry Low -Back Pain Disability Questionnaire (Fairbank et al. 1980) was also developed to assess disability in back pain patients. This questionnaire has 10 items; responses vary from 0 to 5, which indicate different levels of disability regarding daily activities. It takes less than 5 minutes to respond and it is easy to score. Scoring consists of summing the points in each item, total scores will vary from 0 to 50, which is multiplied by two and results are in percentage (Battié and May 2001; Fairbank et al. 1980). The Oswestry Questionnaire has shown moderate correlation with other measures (i.e. the McGill Pain Questionnaire, the RMDQ and the SF-36) and adequate validity and reliability (Battié and May 2001; Roland and Fairbank 2000). However, similarly to the RMDQ, the Oswestry Questionnaire only assesses physical disability. 23 The Medical Outcome Study 36 Short-Form Health Survey (SF-36) (Ware and Sherbourne 1992) has been developed as a generic measure to assess health related quality of life. It has eight scales: physical functioning, role limitations due to physical problems, bodily pain, general health, social functioning, limitation due to emotional problems, vitality and mental health perceptions. The first four scales assess physical health and the last assess mental health. The health concepts from which the scales were developed were selected from forty concepts included in the Medical Outcomes Study (Ware and Sherbourne 1992). The SF-36 is a 5 point rating scale; each domain is scored from 0 to 100, indicating poor and optimal health respectively. It takes approximately 10 minutes to be completed and it is available in a paper and in a computer version. Responsiveness, internal consistency, construct validity and discriminative ability of the SF-36 has been reported by several studies, and its psychometric properties are equal or better than other health related quality of life measures (Bronfort and Bouter 1999; McDowell and Newell 1996; Ware 2000). According to Ware (2000) the physical functioning and the mental health scales are the best all round measures in this area. However in some populations this measure has shown some problems with sensitivity to change (e.g. more disabled patients) (Ruta et al. 1998; Turner et al. 2003). Another issue is that when compared with the RMDQ and the Oswestry Questionnaire, the SF-36 scoring procedure is more complex. Despite these minor problems, the SF-36 has been successfully validated in several populations, including Brazil (Ciconelli et al. 1998). It 24 has also been used in other diseases and health conditions including arthritis and back pain with good results (Ware, 2000). 1.4.2 Comments As noted, in a number of people chronic pain can lead to disability. However, disability is not only a function of physiological aspects and seems to be mediated/moderated by psychosocial factors. Disability self-reports are reliable methods and have been used frequently with chronic pain patients. All measures psychometrically of sound. disability described Furthermore, a in number this of review studies are have compared disability measures and reported that in most cases these measures have a moderate to high correlation, although each measure has its strengths and weaknesses (Donald et al. 1995; McDowell and Newell 1996; Turner et al. 2003). Thus, the selection of a measure depends on its psychometric properties and on the purpose of the assessment (Battié and May 2001). In relation to their content, while the Oswestry and the RMDQ are more specific to some pain conditions, the SIP and the SF-36 are considered generic measures. The main advantages of generic measures are the possibility of comparing disability scores among different groups of patients and being able to assess different spheres of disability. However, there are also a number of studies reporting good results with 25 the adaptation of specific measures to more generic pain conditions (Asghari and Nicholas 2001). Regarding scoring, the RMDQ and the Oswestry Questionnaire are less time consuming and easier to score than the others. Based on a number of findings, the Roland Morris Disability Questionnaire appears to have some advantages over the other measures of disability. It is a specific back pain disability measure successfully adapted to general chronic pain; it has been found to be more sensitive to detect changes in patients with all levels of disability when compared to other disability measures. It is less time consuming and easier to score than all the other measures, except for the Oswestry, and it has a prior validation to the Brazilian population. Furthermore, the concept of disability as measured by the RMDQ is in accordance with WHO concept, focusing on the ability to perform an activity in the manner or within the range considered normal for a human being. The RMDQ modified version (Asghari and Nicholas 2001) has also been used in the Pain Management and Research Centre – RNSH, which will permit comparisons between the studied populations, thus it is the chosen measure to assess disability in this study. In the next chapter evidence about the role of psychological, social and cultural factors participating in physical disability and emotional adjustment associated with chronic pain will be presented. 26 2. Psychosocial Factors Involved in Chronic Pain It has been recognised that psychosocial factors may shape the individual’s pain experience, influencing the degree to which pain is experienced, responses to it, and the degree of interference caused by pain (Linton 2000; Pincus et al. 2002; Skevington 1995; Turk and Okifuji 2002). On the other hand, most evidence is against psychological factors causing chronic pain (Gamsa 1994; Turk and Monarch 2002). Keefe et al. (1990), among others (e.g. Linton, 2000), described a number of factors likely to be involved in the transition from acute to chronic pain. Specifically, Keefe et al. proposed that the acute pain phase is characterised by a focus on somatic symptoms, often a decrease in activities, reliance on medication and beliefs that pain is controllable through medication, seeking professional help, use of passive coping strategies, presence of anxiety and autonomic arousal symptoms. This phase is followed by a pre-chronic phase lasting from 2 to 6 months, which may be characterised by alternating increasing and decreasing activity, withdrawal from reliance on medication, reduced contact with health professionals, working or trying to work, recognising that medication is not always effective, alternating active and passive coping, denying of depression, focus on physical symptoms, pain with varying intensity, and physiological responses similar to the acute pain phase. In Keefe et al.’s model, a chronic pain stage begins after about 6 months, when there is less likelihood of finding an easy solution to organic pathology, or when pathology is diagnosed but may not be possible to 27 treat. At this stage activities usually decrease, a tendency to go from one doctor to another increases, dependence on narcotics may occur, working difficulties and insurance compensation factors may arise. Pain may now be believed to be uncontrollable, depression may be present, passive coping is a common pattern, usually there is a strong preoccupation with bodily complaints, pain is often constant and may be associated with muscular spasm, and in general muscle strength and endurance have decreased. There is increasing evidence supporting much of what Keefe et al. (1990) outlined. In particular, there is evidence that as pain becomes chronic a number of cognitive, psychological and behavioural symptoms and changes become more apparent. Negative mood may arise, passive coping strategies (e.g. resting, avoiding activities that might aggravate pain) are usually predominant, low self-efficacy beliefs and high levels of catastrophising may be present, among other signs of psychosocial maladjustment (e.g. Linton, 2000; Keefe et al., 2004). It has become clear that psychosocial factors can be a consequence of chronic pain or play a role as maintenance or mediating factors (Linton and Skevington 1999; Turk 2002b; Williams 2001). Turk (1996) described that psychological and social factors may act indirectly on pain and disability by reducing physical activity, and consequently muscle flexibility, muscle tone, strength and physical endurance. Cognitive factors may also have a direct effect on physiological parameters, associated more directly with the production or exacerbation of nociception. Cognitive interpretation and affective arousal 28 may directly affect physiology by increasing sympathetic nervous system arousal, production of endogenous opioids (i.e. endorphins) and elevating levels of muscle tension. Altogether, these psychosocial factors seem to contribute to disability. Nicholas (1996) outlined a number of possible interrelationships among biopsychosocial factors, especially in those presenting to pain clinics. R EDUC ED AC TIVIT Y PH YSIC AL DET ER IO RAT IO N (e g. m usc le w asting, jo int s tiffne ss) UN H EL PFU L BEL IEFS & THO U GH TS C HR ON IC PA IN R EPEA TE D TREA TM E NT FAIL U R ES LO N G-T ER M US E OF m ultiple DR UG S FEEL IN GS O F DEP RES SIO N , H ELP LES SN ESS, IR RITA BILITY EXC ESS IVE SU FFER IN G SIDE EFFE CT S (e g. s tom ach pro blem s lethargy, co nstipa tion ) LO SS O F J OB , FIN AN C IAL DIFFIC UL TIE S, FA M ILY ST RE SS Figure 3. Overview of problems associated with chronic pain (Nicholas 1996). This clinical model describes in a very comprehensive way the dynamic interaction among several physiological aspects of pain and emotional, cognitive and behavioural factors. According to this model, reduction of activities and physical deterioration may affect mood (e.g. 29 depression) and cognitions may mediate the relationship between behavioural and emotional variables and vice-versa. Feelings of depression and irritability among other factors may also contribute to reducing activity, treatment failures and social difficulties. Emotions related to stress and social aspects, and physical deterioration may enhance suffering. On the other hand, chronic pain per se may cause or lead to behavioural, cognitive and psychosocial changes. Other interactions as illustrated by the single and double arrows also occur, not only the ones described above (Nicholas 1996). However, while many of those people referred to pain clinics present with this picture, it is also clear that not everyone in the community with chronic pain will present these features. For example, Blyth et al. (2001) found that at least 40% of those with chronic pain reported almost no interference in their daily lives. In an attempt to explain why some become disabled but not others, Turk (2002b) proposed a diathesis-stress model to explain the relationship between trauma and disability. According to this model the impact of a trauma or physical event may depend on predispositional characteristics or traits (e.g. neuroticism). These predispositions are likely to contribute to disability in people that interpret their symptoms in unhelpful ways and that tend to have patterns of response that include anxiety, depression, catastrophic thoughts, fear-avoidance, low-self efficacy and neuroticism. These factors appear to participate as vulnerability factors, which mediate disability. 30 There is also substantial evidence that psychosocial factors play an important role in transition from acute to chronic pain, in the onset and maintenance of chronic pain, in maladjustment to chronic pain, in disability and in psychological dysfunction. Linton’s (2000) review found that psychological factors are linked to the transition from acute to chronic pain and generally have more impact than clinical factors (e.g. pain intensity, pain site). Linton found that the main psychological factors were: attitudes, cognitive style, catastrophising, fear-avoidance, depression, anxiety, distress, passive coping, and self-perceived poor health. Altogether Linton’s findings suggest that psychological factors are important predictors of the risk for developing chronic pain and disability. Pincus et al., (2002) found that among several psychological factors distress and depression are implicated in the transition to chronic pain. This finding confirmed some of Linton’s findings, reinforcing the role of some psychological factors as risk factors for pain chronicity and disability. Although these findings are related to back and neck pain, as they are psychological and social factors there is no obvious reason why the can not be generalised to other chronic pain syndromes and sites. A more recent review (Keefe et al. 2004) highlighted that among several psychological factors recent research suggests that catastrophising, pain related anxiety and fear and helplessness may increase pain, distress and physical disability, while self-efficacy, coping strategies, readiness to change and acceptance may decrease pain, distress and disability. 31 In summary, these reviews of studies examining the possible role of psychological factors in the transition from acute to chronic pain have highlighted a number of contributors to disability and emotional maladjustment associated with chronic pain. As part of this literature review, the specific role of a number of psychosocial factors in physical disability and adjustment to chronic pain will be examined. The role of some cognitive factors (i.e. beliefs and attitudes) will be described, especially self-efficacy, catastrophising and acceptance. The role of depression will also be examined. Finally, this section will examine the role of cultural factors in chronic pain and the issues related to the assessment of psychosocial factors. 32 2.1 Cognitive Factors Biopsychosocial perspectives suggest that a person's cognition about the consequences of an event and ability to respond to it, may have direct and indirect effects on functioning (Flor et al. 1990; Hunt and Ellis 1999). Cognitive processes may be defined as mental processes such as perception, memory, language, concept and representation formation, thoughts, appraisal, beliefs, problem solving. But there are other definitions according to experimental cognitive psychology and cognitive social psychology (Brewin 1988; Hunt and Ellis 1999). Flor et al. (1992) argued that “independent of a medical diagnosis or extent of physical damage, personal evaluations of pain and one’s ability to cope with it are pivotal in determining how disabled a person becomes or remains” (p. 63). Several studies have suggested that changes in maladaptive coping, beliefs and appraisals can contribute to decreased suffering, disability and improved functioning (Flor et al. 1990; Nicholas 1996; Turk and Monarch 2002). In this section some cognitive processes relevant to chronic pain will be described: namely coping, beliefs and attitudes. 33 2.1.1 Coping Strategies Chronic pain and disability can be considered stressors, which in turn mobilise coping strategies. It is generally accepted that coping strategies may act as vulnerability factors and as a mediator of adjustment in a wide range of physical and psychological disorders and events (Brewin 1988; Elliott and Eisdorfer 1982; Flor et al. 1990). Coping may be defined as the use of behavioural and cognitive efforts or purposeful strategies to manage stressful demands that are appraised as taxing or exceeding the resources of a person (Lazarus and Folkman 1984). Coping with chronic pain, can be described as efforts to tolerate, minimise or reduce pain (Robinson et al. 1997). Lazarus and Folkman (1984) conceptualised coping as a process composed of appraisals, responses and reappraisals. Cognitive appraisal and coping are critical processes that mediate the person-environment relationship. Cognitive appraisal may be defined as, “an evaluative process that determines why and to what extent a particular transaction or series of transactions between the person and the environment is stressful. Coping is the process through which the individual manages the demands of a person-environment relationship, that is appraised as stressful and the emotions they generate” (Lazarus and Folkman, 1984, p.19). Lazarus and Folkman (1984) classify cognitive appraisals into primary and secondary appraisals. Primary appraisal consists mainly of giving a meaning to a situation that could be classified in three types: 34 irrelevant, benign-positive and stressful. Secondary appraisal regards the evaluation of what needs to be done, which coping resources are available and possible consequences. A third type of appraisal is reappraisal, and consists of changes in appraisal based on new information acquired. Since the early seventies the role of appraisal in coping and stress has been widely studied. Lazarus and Folkman (1984) and Bennett and Holmes (1975) among others, have demonstrated that cognitive appraisal processes affect stress responses and that appraisal is a strong predictor of chosen coping responses and how people react emotionally. There are a number of different constructs of coping (e.g. emotion and problem focused, active and passive, behavioural and cognitive) and consequently several different measures of coping. Problem-focused and emotion-focused consist of strategies that focus on solving the situation itself or the emotion associated with it, respectively (Lazarus and Folkman 1984). Research based on this conceptual framework has shown that problem-focused coping is associated with lower levels of psychological distress, while emotionfocused strategies are more commonly related to poor adjustment (Lazarus 1993; Robinson et al. 1997). Active coping may be defined as strategies used to attempt to control or function despite pain. Passive coping strategies are characterised by relinquishing control of pain to others (Brown and Nicassio 1987). One of the key issues in this model is whether the patient is relying on internal or external resources to control pain. 35 Behavioural coping strategies consist of actions, behaviours or statements in an effort to deal with chronic pain. Cognitive strategies generally consist of thoughts and emotional oriented strategies (Jensen et al. 1995). Although behavioural strategies appear to be important, Jensen et al. (2003) suggested that cognitive strategies play a more important role in adjustment than behavioural strategies. Illness-focused strategies may be described as efforts to minimise or control the illness effects (e.g. resting), wellness-focused coping strategies consist of activities that supposedly will promote well being (e.g. exercise) (Jensen et al. 1995). Attentional and avoidance pain strategies could be described as another coping conceptual framework. Attentional strategies may include ways of distracting attention from a given situation or object, while avoidance involves diverting the focus from the problem (Holmes and Stevenson 1990). These authors’ findings suggest that chronic pain patients who use attentional coping strategies appear to be less depressed and anxious than patients who use avoidance strategies. Catastrophising is another cognitive construct that has initially been defined as a coping strategy by some authors (Keefe et al., 1989). However, more recently it has been thought of as a maladaptive cognitive response (Turner et al., 2003). The different views on catastrophising have generated some controversy (Jensen et al. 1991b) and will be addressed later. Although the role of coping seems to be important in adjustment to chronic pain, the overlap of constructs makes it difficult to establish a 36 consensus on which types of coping strategies are more efficient and for what type of patient they work (DeGood and Tait 2001). 2.1.1.1 The Contribution of Coping Strategies to Adjustment to Chronic Pain Coping strategies are hypothesised to alter perception of pain, ability to manage it and to continue daily activities. There is evidence that coping strategies can have an impact on function and adjustment to chronic pain and that certain types of coping are related to different outcomes (Jensen et al. 2003; Jensen et al. 1991b; Nicholas et al. 1991; Romano et al. 2003). Although coping is recognised as an important factor in patients’ adjustment to chronic pain several studies have reported varying results. Strategies such as positive self-statements, ignoring pain and increased physical activities have been associated with better psychological status (Jensen and Karoly 1991; Jensen et al. 1991b; Keefe and Williams 1990). Patients who use relaxation techniques also show decreases in pain intensity, psychological distress and functional disability when compared to controls reinterpreting (Flor pain and Turk 1988). and reinterpreting Coping pain self-statements sensations have and been associated with greater perceived control over pain than other strategies (Haythornthwaite et al. 1998). On the other hand, ignoring pain, diverting attention, praying and hoping, resting and guarding have been associated with worse functioning and outcomes (Haythornthwaite et al. 37 1998; Jensen and Karoly 1991; Jensen et al. 1991b; McCracken and Eccleston 2003; Romano et al. 2003). DeGood and Tait (2001) reported that reinterpreting pain has been found to be efficacious to manage chronic pain, but in addition the results or benefits of other coping strategies such as diverting attention, coping self-statements, ignoring pain, praying and increasing activity have not been established and need further research. Furthermore, these authors have argued that different coping strategies may produce different outcomes depending on other moderating variables (e.g. pain intensity, age), under different groups, cultures and conditions, depending on the studyies’ methodologies and on measurement. In general, studies have reported that active coping has been associated with enhanced activity level and better psychological and physical functioning, while passive coping has been associated with maladaptive outcomes (Blyth et al. 2005; Brown and Nicassio 1987; Keefe and Williams 1990; Snow-Turek et al. 1996). Jensen et al. (1995) found that illness-focused coping strategies are related to poorer adjustment to pain and that some wellness-focused strategies were associated with better adjustment to pain. Passive coping strategies and catastrophising appear to be the most relevant coping factors leading to poor outcomes and maladjustment (Boothby et al. 1999; DeGood 2000; Jensen et al. 1995; McCracken and Eccleston 2003; Romano et al. 2003). Only a few studies have failed to find a strong association between catastrophising and disability (Jensen et al., 1992; Jensen et al., 1994). 38 From a clinical perspective, coping strategies appear to be an important factor in adjustment and are frequently addressed through teaching coping skills in cognitive-behavioural therapy. These interventions intend to improve patients’ adjustment towards the development of several coping abilities (Bradley 1996; DeGood and Tait 2001; Williams 2001). This perspective is also supported by DeGood (2000) and Haythornthwaite et al. (1998b) who suggest that high numbers of coping strategies possessed by patients are related with perceived control over pain, suggesting that flexibility contributes to effective coping. Altogether, the majority of the evidence suggests that catastrophising and some passive coping strategies are associated with maladjustment. The literature reviewed also indicates that there is a complex relationship among coping, appraisals and beliefs and adjustment to chronic pain (Boothby et al. 1999; DeGood and Tait 2001; Geisser et al. 1999; Jensen et al. 1991b). To date, as suggested by Lazarus and Folkman (1984) and Lazarus (1993) it seems that effective coping strategy is a function of several factors, such as values, commitments, appraisals, beliefs, personality, and other aspects. A mismatch between coping and some of these factors may reduce the effectiveness of a coping strategy. In the chronic pain field, the role of factors that influence the development of maladaptive coping strategies has been a focus of recent research. This research has suggested that personality traits, specifically 39 neuroticism, seem to predispose some people to engage in catastrophic response to pain. The findings reported by DeGood and Tait (2001), McCracken et al. (2004), Nicholas and Asghari (2006) and Geisser (1999), describe growing evidence for the role of attitudes and beliefs that moderate or mediate coping efficacy, such as readiness for change, flexibility in goal setting, acceptance and self-efficacy. However, much of this work is still relatively preliminary and longitudinal studies, particularly, are needed to clarify the role of these dimensions. As no specific coping strategies have been identified as consistently useful in adapting to chronic pain, this series of studies will focus on the role of catastrophising as a maladaptive response to pain that often mediate adjustment and disability in patients with chronic pain. The concept of catastrophising will be further explored in the next section. 40 2.1.2 Beliefs Beliefs can be defined as pre-existing notions about reality and thus determine how people perceive, evaluate and understand what is happening around them (Wrubel et al. 1981). Fishbein and Ajzen (1975) considered that beliefs consist of information a person has about an object, specifically a belief links an object to some attribute. It implies a person's understanding about him/her and the environment. According to these authors, observation (which will produce descriptive beliefs), information received from outside (that may produce informational beliefs) and inferential processes (responsible for inferential beliefs and based on past experience) will contribute to the formation of beliefs about an object. “The totality of a person’s beliefs serves as the informational base that ultimately determines his/her attitudes, intentions and behaviours“ (Fishbein and Ajzen, 1975, p.14). These authors proposed that only the salient beliefs (the most important beliefs in a certainty moment regarding an object) serve as a determinant of an attitude in a given situation. It is important to differentiate attitudes, beliefs and coping strategies. An attitude may be defined as “a learned predisposition to respond in a consistently favourable or unfavourable manner with respect to a given object” (Fishbein and Ajzen, 1975, p.6). Coping strategies differ significantly from beliefs (which are always mental) in that coping strategies are implicated in efforts and can be overt behaviour. 41 Lazarus (1993) argued that beliefs, along with environmental conditions, influence thoughts, emotions, actions and decisions. In the appraisal processes, beliefs determine what will be considered important, or how people perceive things in the environment and consequently how they might respond. Different types of beliefs have been linked to appraisal. Lazarus and Folkman (1984) suggested that beliefs about personal control (locus of control) and existential beliefs are central in dealing with stressful demands regarding chronic illness. Beliefs about personal control, (i.e. the controllability of a given situation) will also influence emotion and coping. In general it appears that situational factors (e.g. predictability, duration) influence appraisal of an event, but also it seems that the perceived characteristics of the event and personal beliefs play a more important role in the appraisal and coping process (Lazarus and Folkman, 1984). There are a number of types of pain-related beliefs. DeGood and Tait (2001) categorised pain related beliefs into three groups: 1. basic philosophical and ethical assumptions about the nature of the self and the world (e.g. justice, suffering, personal responsibility); 2. generalised beliefs, (e.g. sense of rejection), and 3. specific beliefs regarding the pain experience (e.g. aetiology of pain, self-efficacy beliefs). The third category of belief has been investigated more than the others in chronic pain patients. Several measures have been developed to assess beliefs regarding control, disability, harm, emotion, use of 42 medication, medical cure, self-blame, catastrophising, pain permanence and constancy and self-efficacy (DeGood and Tait 2001; Jensen et al. 1999). This literature review will investigate the role of two of these specific beliefs, namely pain self-efficacy and catastrophising. From a cognitive-behavioural perspective, beliefs are considered important mediators of emotional and behavioural responses to chronic pain (e.g. Turk 2002). There is evidence that changes in beliefs may lead to changes in attitudes, coping and behaviours (Boothby et al. 1999; DeGood and Tait 2001), and that maladaptive beliefs are strongly related to poor adjustment (DeGood 2000; Jensen et al. 1994). To Williams et al. (1994) beliefs have a major role in adjustment to chronic pain. These authors outlined that, pain beliefs “are probably best judged not by how true or false they are, but how adaptive they are in enabling the believer to function in the world he/she experiences” (p. 71.). It has been found that pain-related beliefs play an important role in the transition from acute to disabling chronic pain. Some beliefs may be considered a risk-factor for the development of disability, maladjustment, and poor treatment outcomes (DeGood and Tait 2001; Jensen et al. 1994; Kendall et al. 1997; Linton 2000; Nicholas 1996). For example, beliefs that pain is mysterious, permanent and that the person is to blame have been found to be associated with greater psychological distress. Specifically, pain beliefs regarding mystery appeared to be related to the use of catastrophising and depressive symptoms, while 43 self-blame beliefs have been associated with depressive symptoms. Beliefs about pain constancy have been associated with greater pain severity, and beliefs in pain constancy have been associated with higher anxiety (Turner et al. 2000; Williams et al. 1994). Harm beliefs (beliefs that pain indicates damage and that activity should be avoided) have been associated with poor functioning and pain behaviour (Vlaeyen and Linton 2000). On the other hand, there is evidence that the belief that pain is understandable is associated with higher treatment adherence (Williams et al. 1994). Stronger pain self-efficacy beliefs also have been found to be associated with better adjustment to chronic pain and reduced disability (Asghari and Nicholas 2001; Nicholas and Asghari 2006). Based on these findings it is widely accepted that research and clinical interventions should focus on identifying important dysfunctional and maladaptive beliefs (Jensen et al. 2003). To date one of the key pain beliefs is self-efficacy. It seems to be one of the most relevant to pain adjustment and pain-related disability (DeGood and Tait 2001; Jensen et al. 1991a; Turk and Okifuji 2002). 44 2.1.2.1 Self-efficacy Beliefs Bandura (1977) proposed that efficacy expectancies determine a person’s choice of activities; they affect the person’s willingness to continue in the face of difficulties and adverse situations. Anderson et al. (1995) and Turk (1999) reported that a person’s self-efficacy beliefs regarding a behavioural coping strategy are strongly related to the actual use of a coping behaviour. Bandura (1977) defined self-efficacy as the degree of conviction held by a person that she/he can successfully perform a specific behaviour required to produce a given outcome. Bandura has also described two types of self-efficacy beliefs: outcome expectancy consists of a person’s evaluation that a given behaviour will lead to certain outcomes and efficacy expectations is a person’s conviction that he/she can successfully execute the required behaviour to produce a given outcome. Lazarus and Folkman (1984) and Lazarus (1993) suggested that efficacy expectancies are a secondary appraisal. According to these authors, efficacy expectancies and incentives are inter-related and part of the total evaluation of a situation. They may play a role in evaluating the antecedents and consequences of particular coping strategies. These authors emphasised that “the important point is whether general or specific, illusory or realistic, one’s belief in one’s ability to control an event influences how that event is appraised and, through appraisal, 45 subsequent coping activity” (p.77). Thus, it could be hypothesised that certain beliefs are essential to adaptive coping. Linton and Skevington (1999) proposed that self-efficacy has 3 dimensions rather than the two proposed by Bandura. The first regards expectations about how difficult a task will be; people are more likely to avoid difficult tasks than easy ones. The second and third are more similar to those proposed by Bandura: namely, people may have different expectations about their ability to carry out specific tasks, and how confident a person feels about being able to succeed. Cioffi (2000) described four psychological ways in which selfefficacy beliefs may operate. First, higher self-efficacy levels may decrease anxiety and distress which can be responsible for misinterpretation of physical symptoms; second, an efficacious person may distract attention from unpleasant sensations, third, people with high levels of self-efficacy may also learn other skills to deal with pain and disability and thus persist despite difficulties; and fourth, other meanings can be given to a stimulus previously interpreted as noxious. Linton and Skevington’s (1999) and Cioffi’s (2000) description of the dimensions and psychological mechanisms involved in self-efficacy, emphasise the importance of considering the task and it’s content environment involved (i.e. appraisal of the situation) and perceived selfresources, as aspects considered in relation to the contribution of beliefs to adjustment to chronic pain. 46 2.1.2.1.1 The Contribution of Self-efficacy to Adjustment to Chronic Pain A number of studies have confirmed the relationship between selfefficacy beliefs, disability and adjustment to chronic pain (Arnstein 2000; Jensen et al. 1991a; Nicholas and Asghari 2006; Turk and Okifuji 2002). Anderson et al. (1995) found that self-efficacy beliefs were associated with levels of functioning and response to treatment in a sample of chronic pain patients. Their findings also suggested that patients with higher levels of self-efficacy reported less pain, less interference in daily activities and greater perceived control over pain. Altmaier et al. (1993) reported that increments in self-efficacy predict better functioning and less pain report. Geisser et al. (2003) also found that self-efficacy is related to functional performance. Arnstein et al. (1999) and Arnstein (2000) found that although pain intensity is the most important predictor of disability, self-efficacy mediates the relationship between pain intensity, disability and depression. Their results suggested that lack of self-efficacy beliefs to manage pain, cope and function despite pain, is a significant predictor of disability and depression. Asghari and Nicholas (2001) in a longitudinal study (9 month follow-up) also Questionnaire found similar (Nicholas, results 1989). Their using the results Pain Self-Efficacy showed correlations between low self-efficacy scores, pain, and complaint and avoidance behaviours. They also found that self-efficacy is a stronger predictor of 47 disability than pain and distress. Nicholas and Asghari (2006) findings also suggest that self-efficacy was a significant predictor of disability over and above the effects of age, pain intensity, depression, fear of movement, catastrophising and acceptance of pain. This study also reported that pain severity was significantly predicted by self-efficacy, and also suggested that changes in self-efficacy could be achieved despite no change in pain severity. Nicholas and Asghari (2006) have also reported that self-efficacy is a significant predictor of depression. Anderson et al. (1995) and Rahman et al. (2005) using different measures of self-efficacy (CPSS and PSEQ respectively) found that patients with higher levels of self-efficacy reported a more positive mood and fewer symptoms of depression. Rahman et al. (2005) and Adams and Williams (2003) have also reported that self-efficacy is an important predictor of occupational status. Jensen et al. (1991a) suggested that self-efficacy plays a significant role in the use of different coping strategies among chronic pain patients. This finding is similar to those reported by Turner et al. (2005) with an outpatient community sample with Rheumatoid Arthritis. In this sample, after controlling for age, gender and pain intensity, selfefficacy was associated negatively with disability and depression, and positively with active coping strategies. It has also been recognised that changing beliefs is important in attempts to change behaviour and to re-establish adaptive functioning. Altmaier et al. (1993) and Turner et al. (2007) reported that self-efficacy 48 is an important predictor of adjustment in patients who participate in a CBT intervention program. Turk (2002b) suggested that patients who believe that they can alleviate their suffering are more likely to mobilise resources and skills and to persevere in their efforts to cope with their pain, disability and suffering. On the other hand, a lack of self-efficacy or the belief that there are few possibilities to control pain or functioning despite pain, may lead to a minimal effort to try anything. In turn, this may generate several negative affects, such as depression (Turk and Okifuji 2002). Altogether, the available evidence supports the role of self-efficacy as an important contributor to disability and emotional adjustment to chronic pain. 2.1.2.1.2 Measures of Pain Self-efficacy Beliefs Among several measures that assess self-efficacy beliefs the most commonly used are the Survey of Pain Attitudes - SOPA (Jensen et al. 1987), the Pain Beliefs and Perception Inventory - PBPI (Williams and Thorn 1989), the Chronic Pain Self-Efficacy Scale - CPSS (Anderson et al. 1995) and the Pain Self-Efficacy Questionnaire - PSEQ (Nicholas 1989). However, only the last two measures specifically assess pain self-efficacy beliefs. The Chronic Pain Self-Efficacy Scale – CPSS was developed by Anderson et al. (1995) to measure perceived self-efficacy to cope with the consequences of chronic pain. This measure was based on the 49 Arthritis Self-Efficacy Scale (Lorig et al. 1989). CPSS has 22 items, each rated on a 10-point Likert scale. Factor analyses identified a three factor solution accounting for 64% of the variance. Alpha coefficients for internal reliability of these scales are higher than 0.86 (Anderson et al. 1995). Altogether, data from the initial validation of this measure reported adequate validity and reliability. Anderson et al. (1995) found that three factors comprise the CPSS (i.e. self-efficacy for pain management, self-efficacy for coping with symptoms and self-efficacy for physical function). The three factors were significantly correlated with each other, with other measures such as the Beck Depression Inventory (Beck et al. 1961) and with several scales of the Multidimensional Pain Inventory (Kerns et al. 1985). Anderson et al. (1995) also reported that higher scores on the self-efficacy scales were associated with behavioural coping, less overt pain behaviour, level of functioning, response to treatment, emotional distress and depression. Recently this measure has been validated in Brazil, confirming its psychometric properties and usefulness (Salvetti and Pimenta 2004). The Pain Self-Efficacy Questionnaire – PSEQ (Nicholas 1989) was developed specifically to assess a person’s belief in their ability to function despite pain. It is conceptually based on Bandura’s self-efficacy theory, in the recognition in the reality that in chronic pain, pain relief is rarely possible, and that functioning despite pain is one of the main goals in pain management (Nicholas et al. 1991). The PSEQ has 10 items, reflecting tasks frequently reported as problematic according to patients with chronic pain. Each item is rated in 50 a Likert scale varying from 0 to 6, scaled from not confident at all to completely confident. Scores vary from 0 to 60 and higher scores reflect stronger self-efficacy beliefs. The PSEQ validity was assessed through comparison with other measures with good results. Factor analysis has yielded only one factor and a strong negative correlation between the PSEQ and the RolandMorris Disability Questionnaire (Asghari and Nicholas 1991; Nicholas 2007). The catastrophising scale of CSQ also correlated negatively with PSEQ, as well as other measures, such as Sickness Impact Profile (Deyo 1986), BDI (Beck et al. 1961) and the Pain Beliefs Questionnaire (Gottlieb 1984) (Nicholas, 2007). Ignoring pain, coping self-statement and pain control, measured by CSQ were positively correlated with the PSEQ (Asghari and Nicholas, 1991; Nicholas, 2007). Internal consistency of the PSEQ has been assessed in two studies (both reporting an alpha coefficient of 0.92) (Asghari and Nicholas 1991; Nicholas 2007) and 0.94 (Gibson and Strong 1996). Test re-test correlation coefficients higher than 0.71 (over three months) have been reported in the same studies, which also confirm the stability of this measure. Williams et al. (1993) have also reported that the PSEQ is sensitive to detect changes after pain management interventions, and that its scores are not simply a reflection of pain intensity. Taking all together, these findings provide strong psychometric and theoretical support for the PSEQ, and its ability to assess beliefs relevant to pain management, as well as evaluation of outcomes. Its advantages 51 over the CPSS are that it takes the presence of pain into account and its items are probably more generally applicable across countries. 2.1.2.1.3 Comments Despite the importance of self-efficacy beliefs, there are a few problems in its assessment. These include measurement variability, variability of findings and construct divergences among other problems, that are not within the scope of this review (DeGood and Tait, 2001; Nicholas, 2007). Differences in results reported may be related to the use of different measures with the different dimensions (DeGood and Tait, 2001; Turner et al., 2003; Nicholas, 2007). Most recent findings and studies appear to describe or at least mention the self-efficacy beliefs or pain-related belief concepts that have been investigated, making the scope of the study clearer and clarifying the established relationships among factors. In reference to construct divergences, although there are some conceptual divergences regarding the assessment of self-efficacy beliefs, in general self-efficacy measures assess two domains of this construct: confidence in personal ability to perform specific tasks and confidence to cope with chronic pain (Nicholas, 2007). It is important to note that Bandura’s self-efficacy concept emphasises that the context must be considered when investigating a person’s self-efficacy. Reflection on clinical experience might also suggest 52 that it is one thing to ask a person in pain if they can do something and another thing to ask them if they can do it when they are in pain. Thus, taking the presence of pain into account would seem important when assessing the self-efficacy beliefs of a person in chronic pain. This is consistent with Bandura’s construct. To date only the CPSQ and the PSEQ seem to consider this issue. Problems regarding construct definition measurement variability. Self-efficacy scales may may be also lead to divided into generalised and specific measures. The generalised ones usually assess several domains of beliefs but pay little attention to specific tasks. They may include attitudes, beliefs and coping in the same instrument (i.e. Survey of Pain Attitudes – SOPA, Jensen et al, 1987); they also could assess some pain-related beliefs regarding a specific pathology (e.g. arthritis). On the other hand, specific measures may tend to focus on a construct or on a few tasks hypothesised to be important, such as walking a certain distance. While this is thought to be relevant to that task, it may be difficult to generalise to other tasks or areas of daily life. To date, the literature reviewed suggests that the Pain Self-Efficacy Questionnaire – PSEQ (Nicholas, 1989, 2007) is one of the most relevant measures of this construct. The PSEQ has several advantages over other measures described here. It is a specific measure that focuses on one of the most relevant pain-related beliefs; it is based on a strong theoretical model; its construct is clear and well defined; it takes pain into account when asking patients about their ability to perform a specific task; it has 53 sound psychometric properties and it has been widely used for over a decade in several countries with good results. In addition, the PSEQ is also used at the Pain Management and Research Centre - RNSH, which will permit comparison of the results obtained in a Brazilian population with the population at this pain centre. Based on these considerations, the PSEQ (Nicholas, 1989) is the chosen measure to assess self-efficacy to be used in this research. 54 2.1.2.2 Catastrophising The concept of catastrophising has long been used in relation to depression (Beck 1976; Ellis 1962). Generally, catastrophising has been described in terms of the expectation of the worst possible outcome in a given situation (Beck 1976). Catastrophising was described by Beck (1976) as a cognitive error. Other authors conceptualise it as automatic, unpleasant, magnificatory, and unrealistic interpretation of feared future events (McCracken and Eccleston 2003). Sullivan et al. (1995) stated: “catastrophising involves an exaggerated negative orientation toward a noxious stimulus” (p.2). This conceptual framework views catastrophising as a mental set and that it has at least three dimensions: rumination, magnification and helplessness. Turk (2002b) defines catastrophising as a cognitive coping strategy characterised by negative self-statements and excessive negative future thoughts. There is some conceptual overlap, as well as divergence on conceptual definitions of catastrophising. Some authors define catastrophising as a cognitive coping strategy (Keefe and Williams 1990), others, such as Jensen et al. (1991) and Turner et al. (2000), conceive catastrophising as a cognitive process; an automatic thought, an appraisal. Flor et al. (1992) conceived catastrophising as an automatic thought that may emerge as a self-statement, which is indeed based in cognitive schemata and thus could be defined as a belief. 55 According to Sullivan et al. (1995) the three dimensions of catastrophising; rumination, magnification and helplessness share some features with the concepts of primary and secondary appraisal. However they are also very distinct from effort; which is a central issue in the concept of coping as proposed by Lazarus and Folkman (1984). The conceptual definitions proposed by Sullivan et al. (1995), Jensen et al. (1991) and Flor et al. (1992) are becoming better accepted as many studies are providing evidence regarding the differences among the constructs and relationship with coping strategies (Turner et al. 2000). Most recently catastrophising has been considered by most authors as an appraisal process or a belief rather than a coping strategy (DeGood and Tait 2001; Romano et al. 2003; Stewart et al. 2001). Furthermore, there is evidence suggesting catastrophising is also distinct from depression (Keefe et al. 2004; Stroud et al. 2000; Sullivan et al. 2001a). 2.1.2.2.1 The Contribution of Catastrophising to Adjustment to Chronic Pain In the chronic pain field, catastrophising has been found to be a strong predictor of maladjustment, disability, psychological distress, pain intensity and poor treatment outcomes (Burton et al. 1995; Jensen et al. 2001b; Keefe et al. 2004; Sullivan et al. 2005; Turk and Okifuji 2002). Sullivan et al. (1995) and Sullivan et al. (2004) in experimental studies with students using a cold pressor task to induce pain and 56 assessing catastrophising through the Pain Catastrophising Scale found that high scores on this measure predict intensity, physical and emotional distress. Sullivan et al. (2001a) and Buer and Linton (2002) reported that the relationship between catastrophising and reported pain has been observed in experimental and clinical studies with different pain patients and other specific populations. Asghari and Nicholas (2001) found a relationship between catastrophising and pain intensity in a study with patients having mammography, suggesting that catastrophising is a strong predictor of reported pain severity, with those who respond with catastrophic thoughts reporting higher pain severity. However most of these studies are cross-sectional in design and the direction of influence is unclear. Michael and Burns’ (2004) findings also indicate a relationship between pain intensity and catastrophising and they suggested that catastrophising may affect pain intensity and distress through a bias toward processing the most disturbing aspects of a painful experience. On the other hand, others have reported that pain intensity may influence catastrophising (Jones et al., 2003; Sullivan et al., 2001). In the case of chronic pain at least, as the time course is potentially years, it is likely that the relationship between catastrophising and pain is bidirectional. Several studies have reported that catastrophising is a better predictor of adjustment to pain than coping strategies and medical status variables (Flor and Turk 1988; Keefe and Williams 1990; Lester et al. 57 1996; Rosenstiel and Keefe 1983). Among several psychological variables catastrophising has been found to be a better predictor of disability (Adams and Williams 2003; Sullivan and D'Eon 1990; Sullivan et al. 2005) and quality of life (Lame et al. 2005) than pain intensity. Adams and Williams (2003) have also found that catastrophising is an important predictor of return to work. Keefe et al. (2004) pointed out that catastrophising has emerged as one of the most important predictors of pain, accounting for 7% to 31% of the variance in pain ratings. A number of studies have suggested that catastrophising is more related to the affective dimension of pain. Geisser et al. (1999) suggested that catastrophising may contribute to the evaluative and affective dimensions of pain, which is supported by a number of studies. A study conducted by Turner et al. (2000) found that catastrophising independently predicted depression, but not physical disability. Sullivan et al. (2005) reported that catastrophising was significantly correlated with the affective sub-scale of the MPI. Nicholas and Asghari (2006) also have found that catastrophising was a stronger predictor of depression severity than fear-avoidance, self-efficacy and acceptance of pain. Catastrophising and depression appear to be strongly associated; however the degree of overlap between these constructs is not high enough to consider them redundant (D'eon et al. 2004; Jensen et al. 2001b; Jones et al. 2003; Keefe et al. 2004; Sullivan et al. 2001a). There is also evidence that catastrophising may promote fear of movement which may lead to avoidance behaviour and consequently disuse, physical deconditioning and disability (Vlaeyen and Linton 2000). 58 Sullivan et al. (2001) reported a positive association between catastrophising and illness behaviour, even when controlling mood, neuroticism, pain and disease severity. Catastrophising has also been found to be associated with solicitousness and pain reports (Giardino et al. 2003). These relationships may suggest that catastrophising could contribute to disability and maladjustment by its interactions with other variables. It is expected that other pain beliefs, catastrophising and coping interact dynamically. Turner et al. (2000) suggested that pain beliefs and catastrophising may influence coping, but also that coping in turn will influence beliefs catastrophising and catastrophising. may influence there is These disability finding indirectly suggest through that other appraisals. Although enough evidence of the contribution of catastrophising to disability and adjustment to chronic pain, theoretical refinements of this concept are still in progress. As noted, there is evidence that catastrophising is not a coping strategy, it is also distinct from depression, and may be better defined as a belief or an appraisal process. Sullivan et al. (2001b) described a number of theoretical models (i.e. schema activation, appraisal, attentional and communal coping) that provide a conceptual framework to understand the role of catastrophising in pain. Their review suggests that these models are not incompatible and may offer different contributions to define catastrophising and its 59 dimensions, however do not offer a final solution for the theoretical debate. To Sullivan et al. (2001b), the schema activation and appraisal models offer explanations to understand processes and mechanisms that underlie the relationship attentional processes), between but do not catastrophising explain the and pain development (i.e. and maintenance of catastrophising. The communal coping model emphasises more the contribution of social-behavioural aspects to catastrophising. 2.1.2.2.2 Catastrophising Measures This literature review has found that the most used measures to assess catastrophising are the Catastrophising Scale of Coping Strategies Questionnaire Catastrophising – CSQ Scale (Rosenstiel - PCS and (Sullivan Keefe et al. 1983), 1995) the and Pain the Catastrophising Scale of the Pain-Related Self-Statements - PRSS (Flor et al. 1993). The Coping Strategies Questionnaire (Rosenstiel and Keefe, 1983) is a 44-item self-report questionnaire which consists of 8 subscales: reinterpreting pain sensations, positive coping self-statements, ignoring pain sensations, increase activity level, diverting attention, increasing pain behaviours, praying and hoping, catastrophising scale and two selfefficacy items. The catastrophising scale has 6 items that reflect elements of helplessness and pessimism in relation to pain experience. 60 There are several studies reporting the psychometric properties of this measure, however, the subscale structure of the CSQ as a whole has been found to be inconsistent or at least problematic. Nevertheless, the psychometric properties of the catastrophising scale have been found to be stronger than the other scales of the CSQ. It has been reported this scale has good construct validity, reliability and stability (DeGood and Tait 2001; Geisser et al. 1994; Jensen et al. 1991b; Stewart et al. 2001). Stewart et al. (2001) reported an inadequate internal consistency, and low test-retest reliability (0.7) on all scales, except for catastrophising. On the other hand, the catastrophising scale has shown a significant correlation with other catastrophising measures (r=0.74), such as the Cognitive Coping Strategy Inventory (Stewart et al., 2001). Studies using the CSQ have found that catastrophising is one of the strongest predictors of several outcomes of maladjustment (DeGood and Tait 2001; Geisser et al. 1994; Jensen et al. 1991b; Stewart et al. 2001). There are several criticisms regarding the definition of catastrophising as a coping strategy and a sub-scale of CSQ. As noted before, due to conceptual divergence several authors dispute the idea that catastrophising is not a coping strategy (Jensen et al., 1991a; Turner et al., 2000). Beside this divergence, CSQ also seems to have construct problems. Several findings do not agree with the number of CSQ factors, suggesting the existence of three, five or six factors, as well as the fact that the catastrophising scale is the only CSQ value scale. Furthermore, 61 there is also divergence on CSQ scoring procedures (Hadjistavroupolos et al. 1999; Jensen et al. 1995; Robinson et al. 1997; Romano et al. 2003). Despite these concerns, the CSQ is one of the most widely used measures, and several findings about the role of coping and catastrophising in adjustment were produced using the CSQ. The PCS - Pain Catastrophising Scale is a thirteen-item scale developed by Sullivan et al. (1995) to assess catastrophising thoughts related to chronic pain. PCS items were derived from catastrophising ideation symptoms and from the CSQ (Rosenstiel and Keefe 1983). The items are rated in a 5-point scale, varying from 0 to 4, and ranging from the words “not at all” to “all the time”. On the validation of the PCS, Sullivan et al. (1995) found three scales (rumination, magnification and helplessness) with moderate intercorrelations and high internal consistency suggesting that there are different dimensions of this construct. Other studies have confirmed the existence of three factors, reliability and validity of the PCS (Osman et al. 2000; Sullivan et al. 2004), as well as high test-retest correlations (0.70 – 0.80) (Sullivan et al., 2001). The authors also found differences in scores associated with gender; females tend to score higher on the rumination and helplessness scales than males, but these differences were not confirmed in later studies (Sullivan et al. 2001). Other studies, have found a two-factor model of the PCS (rumination and powerlessness, which is a combination of magnification and helplessness) (Chibnall and Tait 2005). 62 Another measure used to assess catastrophising is one of the scales of the Pain-Related Self-Statements (Flor et al., 1993). This measure was developed based on the concepts of cognitive schemata and automatic thoughts. Cognitive schemata may be defined as “cognitive structures that contain the individual’s stored knowledge about the world, and they guide the organisation of incoming information” (Flor et al., 1993, p. 64). Automatic thoughts are a product of cognitive schemata and may emerge in the form of self-statements regarding a specific situation. Cognitive schemata are formed based on past experience as well as beliefs and thus will mediate a person’s pain perception and response to it. Indeed, the concept of cognitive schemata is similar to the concept of cognitive configurations, a central concept in the definition of belief. Cognitive schemata are cognitive processes that occur prior to cognitive appraisal, thus influencing appraisal. This assumption is in accordance with Lazarus and Folkman’s (1983) concept of coping, which recognised the influence of beliefs and other factors in coping strategies. The development of the PRSS is based on the assumption that these cognitions play a role in promoting or undermining attempts to cope with pain, and that coping measures such as the VPMI and CSQ did not properly assess these constructs. PRSS items were derived from a clinical setting and were factor analysed, and yielded a 9-item scale for catastrophising rated on a 6point scale (0 to 5). Construct validity, reliability, stability and sensitivity properties were reported by Flor et al. (1993). Factor analysis of the two 63 scales of the PRSS (catastrophising and coping) reported a lower correlation between them, suggesting that they are distinct constructs and should be analysed separately. The authors also suggested that the more negative scales, helplessness and catastrophising are more crucial to understand other pain variables. Meanwhile passive coping selfstatements and resourcefulness seem not to be crucial factors (Flor et al., 1993). This scale has been used at the Pain Management and Research Centre for over 10 years and its psychometric properties have been found to be good (Nicholas 2007; Nicholas and Asghari 2006). 2.1.2.2.3 Comments Catastrophising has emerged as one of the most robust psychological predictors of pain outcomes (Sullivan et al. 2001a; Thorn et al. 2003). As noted, catastrophising is associated positively with measures of physical disability and psychological adjustment, as well as pain severity in patients with different pain conditions. Despite the demonstrated importance of this construct as a mediator factor in adjustment to chronic pain, Turner and Leslie (2001) and Sullivan et al. (2001) identified a number of key issues still to be explored regarding the assessment of catastrophising: construct validity, stability, relationship with other factors and establishing causal relations. Most catastrophising measures appear to be related but Turner and Leslie (2001) questioned whether they capture the original construct 64 proposed by Ellis (1962) and Beck (1976). In their study, Turner and Leslie (1994) reported that these measures seem to have good face, content and construct validity and are distinct from the concept of anxiety and depression, but the measures may be improved if they explored a wider range of items. They suggested that the CSQ and the PCS would increase their validity if more items were added. To date, there are no studies comparing these three measures, but it appears that approximately half of the PRSS items share the same content as the PCS items. Furthermore the PRSS catastrophising scale explores a few broader contents and is more pain-specific than the CSQ, one of the most used catastrophising measures. The second aspect proposed by Turner and Leslie (2001) regards the definition of catastrophising as a state or a trait, a dispositional factor or a situational response. Studies using different measures of catastrophising have reported a high test-retest correlation. CSQ showed a (0.81) test-retest correlation (Brown et al. 1989), PCS had a (0.75) correlation across a 6-week period (Sullivan et al. 1995), while the PRSS had a (0.87) correlation coefficient (Flor et al. 1993). These findings somehow support the evidence that catastrophising is stable; and could be defined as a personality trait. On the other hand, Sullivan et al. (1995) reported that catastrophising scores have changed after some interventions and certain conditions (e.g. affective states), which support the evidence that catastrophising may be determined by situational factors. However, a third and intermediate alternative has been proposed. This suggests that, catastrophising may be considered a stable 65 tendency, which may vary as a function of certain conditions (Sullivan et al., 2001; Turner and Leslie; 2001). This issue may be extremely important from a clinical point of view, as interventions intend to change patterns aiming to increase adjustment. The relationship between catastrophising and other factors (i.e. beliefs, depression) is becoming better understood. Although there is some evidence that catastrophising predicts depression (Jensen et al., 1991; Nicholas and Asghari, 2006)), further research needs to be done to understand the role of catastrophising as a mediator of adjustment to chronic pain and the interaction between catastrophising and other variables. Improvement of catastrophising measures, longitudinal studies and cross-cultural research may collaborate to increase the knowledge in this field. The evidence collected in this literature review has provided theoretical support to choose the Catastrophising scale of the PainRelated Self-Statements (Flor et al., 1993) as a measure to assess catastrophising. The PRSS has equal or superior psychometric properties when compared to the CSQ and the PCS. There is not an overlap of other constructs in this instrument. Its items are more pain-specific than the CSQ and the PCS, which may provide important information. Furthermore the content of the PCS items seems to be assessed by the PRSS catastrophising scale. The PRSS catastrophising scale is also the measure used at the Pain Management and Research Centre – PMRC to assess catastrophising, which would allow a comparison between data collection from a Brazilian 66 population and the population at this centre. Therefore, the PRSS catastrophising scale is the chosen measure of this series of studies. 67 2.2 Attitudes Attitude has been defined as “a learned predisposition to respond in a consistently favourable or unfavourable manner with respect to a given object” (Fishbein and Ajzen, 1975, p.6). Attitudes may also be conceptualised as a tendency to react in a certain manner to a given situation. They are tightly related to beliefs and appraisals, and also share some conceptual bases. One of the key differences between beliefs and attitudes is that the latter are always associated with a response or orientation towards an object. Although there are several categories of attitudes, this series of studies will focus only on acceptance. 2.2.1 Acceptance of Chronic Pain Acceptance may be described as an active and aware attitude, which implies a willingness to remain in contact and experience pleasant or unpleasant thoughts, emotions, feelings and sensations without trying to change or avoid them (Hayes et al. 1994). This concept is not a novel concept in some fields of Psychology. In Gestalt therapy acceptance and awareness are central processes in psychotherapy and the bases for any change (Perls 1969). Kübler-Ross (1982) described acceptance as one of the stages of loss, indeed an important stage in dealing with any grief and loss. Hayes et al. (1999) suggested that acceptance may be considered the opposite of avoidance and different from tolerance. 68 In western societies, this concept might be frequently misunderstood and confounded with passivity, and not considered an important feature of adjustment. However, recently a number of studies have described the importance of acceptance to adjustment to several chronic illnesses including chronic pain (Hayes et al. 2006; McCracken and Eccleston 2003; McCracken and Eccleston 2006; Viane et al. 2004). The concept of acceptance is quite recent in the chronic pain literature, but it may make an important contribution to this field, especially because, for most chronic pain patients, there is no medical procedure to permanently relieve pain (Turk 2002a). In this context, there may be adaptive advantages in not persisting with efforts to avoid what is not possible to be avoided, as it could lead to excessive distress, anxiety, depression and other negative affects (Hayes et al. 2006). Acceptance of chronic pain implies living with pain without useless reactions and attempts to control or avoid it. According to Hayes et al. (1999) and McCracken and Eccleston (2003), acceptance involves acknowledging pain, giving-up unproductive attempts, or disengagement from struggling with pain, acting as if pain does not necessarily imply disability, developing a realistic approach to pain and pain-related circumstances, and engagement in positive everyday activities despite pain. Acceptance also involves a neutral acknowledgment frame and willingness to live with pain, which is not tolerance and also different from passive coping strategies which usually lie in external resources to manage pain. Acceptance also implies not avoiding activities that may 69 cause pain, and it can be differentiated from self-efficacy, because it does not consider managing pain (McCracken et al. 2004a). Acceptance seems to have a few dimensions. Risdon et al. (2003) found 8 different accounts of acceptance, but reported that all the features could be grouped in three major dimensions: 1. focus away from pain to no-pain aspects of life, 2. recognition that cure would be unlikely to happen, and 3. the rejection that acceptance is equal to failure. McCracken and colleagues analysing the Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire (McCracken, 1998) and the Illness Cognition Questionnaire (Evers et al. 2001) reported that these measures have a moderate correlation and therefore may measure distinct components of acceptance. Furthermore, analyses of the CPAQ suggest the existence of four factors: activity engagement, pain willingness, thought control and chronicity. But only the first two seem to have adequate psychometric properties and correlate well with relevant outcomes (McCracken et al, 1999; McCracken et al, 2004b; Viane et al, 2003), therefore two major dimensions seem to better describe this construct. 2.2.1.1 The Contribution of Acceptance to Adjustment to Chronic Pain Several studies have found significant associations between acceptance and adjustment to chronic pain. This section will cover the main features regarding this relationship. Experimental studies have found that participants with higher levels of acceptance report higher tolerance to induced pain and less pain 70 intensity than the placebo and control group (Hayes et al. 1999). Other studies found that perceived control over pain had a smaller correlation with better functioning and suggested that the adoption of an accepting attitude to pain may promote a higher sense of self-control (Jacob et al. 1993; Tan et al. 2002). Clinical studies have found that greater acceptance of pain is associated with lower pain intensity, less pain related anxiety, avoidance, less depression, disability, up time and better work status. After controlling for demographic variables and pain intensity, acceptance predicted better adjustment than all the other variables described above (McCracken, 1998). McCracken and Eccleston (1999) and McCracken et al. (1999) reported that dysfunctional patients (e.g. not well adapted to chronic pain) have greater pain-related anxiety and less acceptance of pain than adaptive patients, and that anxiety and acceptance contributed uniquely to adjustment independent of depression and pain intensity. Based on these findings, they suggested that decreasing anxiety and increasing acceptance might help patients to become more adaptive and adjusted to chronic pain. Furthermore, results from these studies have also shown relatively low correlations between acceptance and pain intensity, suggesting that acceptance is not simply a function of having low pain intensity. Other studies done by McCracken and Eccleston (2003a, 2006) found that greater acceptance of chronic pain was associated with less pain intensity, lower levels of disability, depression and pain-related anxiety, uptime and working status. When compared to coping, 71 acceptance accounted for more variance in adjustment than coping strategies. In their second study, they also reported that acceptance is a more reliable predictor of stress and disability than coping. Acceptance also has been found to be associated with mental wellbeing, and seems to be a more important predictor of it than pain intensity and catastrophising (Viane et al. 2003). Viane et al. (2004) also reported that acceptance seems to be associated with less attention to pain and engagement in meaningful life activities. Nicholas and Asghari (2006) also investigated the role of acceptance considering the CPAQ scales and found that only the activity engagement scale predicts depression, but neither of the subscales predict pain intensity and disability. Also when compared to self-efficacy and catastrophising, acceptance appears to be a less important predictor of disability than self-efficacy. Results from this study found that acceptance was catastrophising, a and less also important less predictor important of than depression than self-efficacy and catastrophising in predicting pain intensity. When analysing the predictive properties of the four factors, these authors found that only the first factor (i.e. activity engagement) contributes to depression. Other studies have shown that activity engagement and pain willingness have singular roles. McCracken and Eccleston (2003) reported that activity engagement is a stronger predictor than pain willingness on several outcomes (i.e. physical and psychosocial disability, uptime, work status and depression). In another study McCracken and colleagues (2004a) reported that only the pain willingness factor accounted for a 72 significant variance in pain intensity, medical visits, physical disability and depression, while the activity engagement factor accounted only for work status. However when applied as distinct scales, they both predicted depression, pain-related anxiety and psychosocial adjustment, but only pain willingness accounted for a significant variance in physical disability. Viane et al. (2003) also failed to find an association between acceptance and physical disability. Taken together, these results suggest that acceptance seems to be an important predictor of physical and psychological adjustment. However, studies have yielded different results which suggest that findings with the CPAQ are not robust. So far, it seems that the activity engagement scale is more relevant than pain willingness, especially on psychological outcome (i.e. depression). From a clinical perspective, CBT based programs for chronic pain often incorporate the concept of avoidance as one of their main principles. This aspect may be somehow considered the opposite of the concept of acceptance which implies acknowledging the pain, and carrying on with daily activities aiming to avoid disability. This modality of interventions frequently emphasise the importance of not denying pain, improving physical and psychosocial functioning and not focusing solely on pain intensity reduction. Geisser et al. (1994) found that greater acceptance of pain at the end of a behavioural treatment predicted less interference in daily activity and higher levels of activity. More recently a few studies have tested the importance of acceptance based interventions and found that patients 73 that have greater acceptance scores showed significant improvements in emotional, social and physical outcome (McCracken et al, 2005). Recent studies of catastrophising and avoidance also suggest some relationship among these features and hypothesise that enhancing acceptance may reduce catastrophic thoughts and avoidance (McCracken and Eccleston 2003; Vlaeyen and Linton 2000). Nicholas (2007) hypothesised that there is a possible relationship between acceptance and self-efficacy, suggesting that if a person has accepted their chronic pain, the chances of engaging in activities would be greater but still influenced by his/her pain self-efficacy beliefs. Viane et al. (2003) point out that although it is recognised that it is important to develop coping strategies to deal with pain, it appears that strategies to control pain did not reduce depression, and were not related with better social functioning in patients with high-intensity pain. Actually according to this perspective, attempts to control chronic pain may be maladaptive. This finding is in accordance with McCracken et al. (2003), who found that adaptive copers showed greater acceptance than dysfunctional patients, and McCracken and Eccleston (2006) who found that acceptance is a better predictor of adjustment than coping. Altogether, these findings indicate that exploring the role of acceptance may contribute to improve the understanding of adjustment to chronic pain. Furthermore, these results may suggest that acceptance of pain might be an important mediator of adjustment for some chronic pain patients. 74 2.2.1.2 Acceptance Measures Consistent with the limited evidence regarding the role of acceptance in chronic pain is the scarcity of its measures. The only measures found on this literature review were the Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire (Geiser 1992) and the Illness Cognition Questionnaire (Evers et al. 2001). The Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire – CPAQ (Geiser 1992) was developed to assess acceptance of pain based on an early version of the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire (Hayes, 2006). It has 34 items rated in a Likert scale from 0 to 6 ranging from "never true" to "always true". The initial study described medium to moderate correlations of this measure with measures of depression, anxiety and disability, supporting CPAQ’s validity. McCracken (1998) proposed a 24-item version, using only the items found to be relevant. This version has been reported to have a positive item-correlation and internal consistency (0.84). Initially a four structure factor was proposed: engaging in normal life activities; believing that controlling thoughts controls pain; recognising pain chronicity and needing to avoid or control pain (McCracken et al. 1999). However, further studies have analysed the factor structure of the CPAQ, proposing an elimination of some irrelevant items, confirming its face validity and suggesting minor variations on the existent four factors (McCracken et al. 2004b). The four factors proposed were: activity engagement (attitudes that reflect the need to engage in or pursue in 75 normal life activities while pain is being experienced); pain willingness (recognition that avoidance and control often do not work to adapt to chronic pain); thought control and chronicity (recognising that pain may not change). These findings suggest that only the activity engagement and pain willingness were significant predictors of pain-related disability and distress (McCracken et al. 2004b). This latest study confirmed the psychometric properties of the Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire for a 20-item structure and confirmed the existence of two major components. The first scale named activity engagement consists of items that reflect the need to engage in or pursue activities despite pain, and the second scale concerns the recognition that avoiding or controlling pain may be of a little use and an ineffective strategy. The Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire – CPAQ (20-item version) proposed by McCracken et al. (McCracken et al. 2004b) appears to be an adequate measure with good psychometric properties to assess chronic pain acceptance. The Illness Cognition Questionnaire - ICQ (Evers et al. 2001) was developed to assess cognition reflecting meanings of chronic diseases. It has 18-items, which yield three general illness cognitions: helplessness, acceptance and disease benefits. The validity of the ICQ has been supported by its correlations with measures of physical and psychological health status (Evers et al., 2001; Viane et al., 2003). Although the ICQ was not developed to assess acceptance regarding chronic pain, it has a 76 moderate correlation with the CPAQ, which suggests that they may measure different aspects of acceptance (Viane et al. 2003). 2.2.1.3 Comments The concept of acceptance is novel and promising in relation to adjustment to chronic pain. Although there is some evidence of the role of acceptance in chronic pain, further research is needed to clarify its role and relationships with other factors, such as beliefs, coping strategies, disability and adjustment to chronic pain, and to assess the contribution of acceptance to physical disability and emotional adjustment (McCracken and Eccleston 2006; Nicholas and Asghari 2006; Sullivan et al. 2005; Viane et al. 2003). Recent findings from these authors enhanced the importance of clarifying the definition of acceptance, improved acceptance measures and providing evidence about the role of acceptance in disability and adjustment. In relation to conceptual definitions, generally, acceptance could be conceived as either an attitude or a belief. According to Fishbein and Aizen (1975), an attitude is a learned predisposition to respond in a consistently favourable or unfavourable way to a given object, while beliefs are defined as pre-existing notions about reality which serve as a perceptual lens and thus determine how a person evaluates and understands what is happening (Wrubel et al. 1981). To Fishbein and Ajzen (1975), beliefs consist of information a person has about an object, 77 and links an object to some attribute. It implies a person' understanding about himself/herself and the environment. Although attitudes may be based on beliefs, they imply an orientation towards an object, thus it seems more appropriate to define acceptance as an attitude. Recent studies (McCracken et al. 2004a; Risdon et al. 2003; Viane et al. 2003) have applied the concept of acceptance initially proposed by Hayes et al. (1994) by considering the context of chronic pain sufferers. This novel conceptualisation regarding acceptance in chronic pain emphasises the attitude to shift from pain to no-pain aspects of life and the need to acknowledge that there might not be a total cure for pain (Risdon et al. 2003). It may also be important to differentiate acceptance from other constructs, especially self-efficacy. Self-efficacy concerns beliefs related to the ability to manage pain, while acceptance is more related to facing chronic pain without judging it or coping with it. Another important aspect regarding the concept of acceptance is related to its dimensions. As noted previously, studies have reported a moderate correlation between two measures of acceptance. Furthermore, studies have reported different findings about the contribution of acceptance to pain intensity, disability and emotional adjustment, as well as the unique contribution of its factors. These findings suggest that acceptance is composed of more than one dimension, and these dimensions probably play different roles in physical disability and emotional adjustment. 78 The relationship between acceptance and pain-related beliefs (e.g. self-efficacy and catastrophising), and acceptance and mood, and what are their unique roles in disability and adjustment is not well understood yet, and further research should address these questions. Altogether, the evidence gathered suggests that although acceptance seems to predict physical and psychological adjustment, studies have yielded different results, which suggest that those findings are not robust. So far, it seems that the activity engagement scale is more relevant than pain willingness, especially when evaluating psychological outcome (e.g. depression). Therefore, further studies might be necessary to support the psychometric properties of this measure, as well as providing more evidence about the contribution of acceptance and its dimensions to adjustment. In this series of studies the psychometric properties of the CPAQ will be examined in a non-English speaking population, which may help to improve our understating of the use of the CPAQ. Furthermore, the contribution of acceptance to physical and emotional adjustment will be evaluated in a Latin American population with chronic pain, which may shed some light on the role of acceptance in other non English speaking populations with chronic pain. Beside these features, this series of studies will also compare the contribution of acceptance to disability including measures of self-efficacy, depression and catastrophising. Supported by these assumptions, and aiming to understand the role of acceptance in disability and its relationship with other factors, this series of studies choose the Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire – 79 CPAQ (20-item version) (McCracken et al., 2004b) as the measure to assess acceptance in chronic pain patients. The CPAQ (McCracken et al., 2004) has stronger psychometric properties than the Illness Cognition Questionnaire (Evers et al. 2001); it was specifically developed for chronic pain patients, its factors appear to be more in consonance with relevant findings reported by the literature and it is widely used. As a part of this research project, the CPAQ was added to the battery of measures used in the Pain Management and Research Centre – RNSH, making it possible to compare the samples investigated in this series of studies. 80 2.3 Affective Factors Among all affective factors associated with chronic pain, depression appears to be one of the most relevant, as well as the most studied. Depression is more common among chronic pain patients than in the general population and healthy controls (Pincus and Williams 1999; Romano and Turner 1985; Turk and Okifuji 2003). Although the prevalence of depression varies among different chronic pain populations, rate of depression in this population is considered high. Reviews have reported a depression prevalence range from 1.5% to 57% (Fishbain et al. 1997; Worz 2003). McWillians et al. (2003) have found a depression prevalence of 20% in a chronic pain non-clinical representative sample in the United States. The large variance in results may be due to different diagnostic criteria, measures and setting. Despite different results, from a clinical perspective the association of depression and chronic pain is considered very important. 2.3.1 The Contribution of Depression to Disability Associated with Chronic Pain Worz (2003) suggest that depressed chronic pain patients are less active than non-depressive patients, that depression reduces the likelihood of responding to treatment and worsens the effect of chronic pain on physical and psychological functioning. 81 A literature review by Pincus et al. (2002) found that depression, anxiety and somatization are implicated in transition from acute to chronic low-back pain. According to Pincus and Williams (1999) selfreports of depression predict disability better than pain intensity and pain duration. Keefe et al. (2001) and Pincus and Williams (1999) have suggested that depression may not be only associated with chronic pain, indeed it is a risk factor to pain onset and development, as well as disability. Haythornthwaite et al. (1991) reported that chronic pain patients with depression reported greater pain intensity, greater pain interference, and more pain behaviours. They also found that depressed patients are more likely to drop out of a rehabilitation program. Keefe et al. (2001) findings suggest that emotional distress (i.e. depression) has being associated with higher numbers of treatment seeking behaviour. Breslau et al. (2003) found that higher disability levels related to headache were associated with depression. Nicholas (1996) pointed out that depression contributes to suffering, disability, as well as impaired coping abilities (Von Korff et al. 1993). Banks and Kerns (1996) have found that depression may mediate the relationship between chronic pain and disability. On the other hand, other studies have reported that physical disability, self-efficacy and catastrophising could also mediate depression (Asghari 1996). Arnstein et al. (1999) have also reported that selfefficacy and pain intensity contributed to the development of depression and disability. 82 The influence of depression on chronic pain has also been associated with pain intensity, gender and age. Jensen et al. (1994) found that pain intensity was a significant predictor of depression. Turk et al. (1995) found that in older patients there is a strong association between pain severity and depression, not observed in younger patients. Other findings suggest that although gender does not predict disability, it moderates the relationship between depression and disability (Campbell et al. 2003; Keogh et al. 2006). Evidence of the relationship between pain intensity, disability and depression has been well reported in the literature, however with a large variance (McCracken and Eccleston 2005; Turk et al. 1995). Gender and age have also often been reported as moderators of depression, again with a large variance in results (Harris et al. 2003). A few theoretical models have described the contribution of depression to chronic pain and disability (e.g. Pincus and Willians, 1999; Turk, 2002b). According to these models depression may contribute to disability due to interaction with other factors (e.g. anxiety, catastrophic thoughts, fear-avoidance, low self-efficacy and neuroticism). These factors appear to participate as vulnerability factors to disability. Although the role of depression in chronic pain is recognised, the nature of this relationship remains unclear. Robinson and Riley (1999) described the existence of four models regarding the relationship of negative affect and pain. The first perspective suggests that negative emotions increase pain report through the sensory dimension (e.g. increasing sensitivity to noxious stimuli). Another model posits that pain 83 is caused by negative emotions (e.g. an underlying depressive disorder). A third hypothesis suggests that negative emotion is a common response to pain, while a fourth hypothesis proposes that chronic pain and depression occur simultaneously, due to similar physiological mechanisms. Broadly these models could be described as antecedent, consequence and bidirectional hypotheses respectively (Worz 2003). A review by Fishbain et al. (1997) reported that 9 of 13 studies that endorsed the pain as a consequence hypothesis failed to support it with evidence, while another 15 studies supported the hypothesis that depression may follow chronic pain. Evidence gathered by Gamsa (1994) suggests that depression may often be a consequence of chronic pain that is also mediated by other factors (e.g. self-efficacy), but which in turn mediates other factors such as coping, physical and psychological functioning. To Worz (2003) the third hypothesis (i.e. negative emotion is a common response to pain) needs further evidence, and it seems more appealing that the relationship of depression and chronic pain is dynamic and bidirectional. A model proposed by Pincus and Williams (1999) illustrates the multi-directional relationship between depression and other factors in chronic pain. 84 Stressor Stressor Pain Injury Trauma DIATHESIS Negative Affect (Depression) Disability Cognitive Vulnerability Stressful Life Events Figure 4. A Multistage model of cognition, disability and affect (Pincus and Williams 1999). To date it seems that no single model that has been widely accepted to explain the relationship between chronic pain and depression. As proposed by Worz (2003), a multifactorial and bidirectional comprehension of this relationship seems to be better supported by evidence and is more adequate from a clinical perspective as well. Taken together, these findings suggest that depression may mediate the perception of noxious stimuli, as well as responses to chronic pain, which in turn may increase disability and maladjustment to chronic pain. On the other hand, depression does not appear to be an important predictor of physical disability, especially when compared to other factors such as catastrophising, self-efficacy and acceptance. 85 2.3.2 Depression Measurement Despite the prevalence and contribution of depression to chronic illness and specifically to chronic pain there is criticism regarding the construct validity of a number of measures of depression. Debate has emerged over the actual nature of depression in chronic pain populations, especially in relation to symptoms that may be shared between chronic illnesses, pain, side-effects of treatment and depression (e.g. Pincus and Williams, 1999). A number of researchers (e.g. Lovibond and Lovibond, 1995; Morley et al., 1995; Novy et a., 1995; Pincus and Williams, 1999) have explored cognitive models of depression, in contrast to the more traditional DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association 1994) which describes depression in terms of somatic, affective and cognitive symptoms. These different models of depression have implications for the assessment of depression, especially when assessment is based on older psychometric scales (e.g. Beck (Beck et al. 1961), Zung (Zung and Durham 1965) and Hamilton (Hamilton 1960). These older scales are derived from the traditional DSM approach and, accordingly, include somatic symptoms, such as: insomnia, appetite changes, weight loss, loss of libido, and motor retardation (Aylard et al. 2005; Beck et al. 1961; Hamilton 1960; Heretik and Molcan 1984; Zung and Durham 1965). The somatic items inflates depression scores in chronic pain populations, making its diagnosis and assessment biased (Pincus and Williams 1999; Robinson and Riley III 1999; Taylor et al. 2005). 86 Pincus and Williams (2004) found that chronic pain patients tend to endorse the somatic items of depression more often than depressed people without chronic pain. In this population, it seems that somatic items may be relatively poor markers of depression; chronic pain patients may experience somatic symptoms for other reasons than depression; and that the presence of somatic items may inflate test scores in this population (Pincus et al. 2004; Taylor et al. 2005). To date, the most widely used measures to assess depression in chronic pain patients are the Beck Depression Inventory – BDI (Beck et al. 1961), the Center of Epidemiological Study-Depression Scale – CES-D (Radloff 1977) and the Zung Self-rating Depression Scale (Zung and Durham 1965). However, to overcome the problem of bias caused by somatic items in the assessment of depression, especially in medical populations, a number of alternative depression measures have been used. These include the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) (Zigmond and Snaith 1983), the DASS - Depression, Anxiety and Stress Scales (Lovibond and Lovibond 1993), the Depression, Anxiety, and Positive Outcome Scale (DAPOS) (Pincus et al. 2004). Below the most used measures of depression will be briefly described. The Center of Epidemiological Study-Depression Scale - CES-D (Radloff 1977) was developed to measure levels of depressive symptomatology, with a focus on affective, somatic and interpersonal aspects of depression. The CES-D has 20 items; rated in a four point scale, and a cut-off of 16 points for general population while a cut-off of 87 19 has been suggested for chronic pain patients (Turk et al. 1995). Radloff (1977) has described adequate validity and reliability properties for the CES-D. Other studies have confirmed the psychometric properties of this measure and pointed out that it has fewer somatic items than the BDI (Bradley and McKendree-Smith 2001; Turk et al. 1995). The Beck Depression Inventory – BDI (Beck et al., 1961) was based on Beck’s depression theory and its items were selected partially from DSM-III. Originally the BDI was developed to evaluate cognitive components of depression; however, it has several somatic items (e.g. sleep disturbance). A 3 factor structure has been reported (i.e. negative attitudes/suicide, performance difficulty and physiological manifestation) (Novy et al. 1995; Turk et al. 1995) which is not discrepant with BDI theoretical bases . The BDI has 21 items, rated on a 0 to 3 scale, with wellestablished means for a number of groups, including chronic pain patients. The BDI psychometric properties have been supported by many studies, and this is probably the most widely used measure to assess depression (Bradley and McKendree-Smith 2001; Robinson and Riley III 1999). There is also a Brazilian validation of this measure for general population and for chronic pain patients (Cunha 2001). Studies comparing the BDI and CES-D suggest that both measures have adequate sensitivity and specificity properties. The CES-D seems to be more sensitive to detect change than the BDI, while the BDI seems to be more specific to detect depression (Bradley and McKendree-Smith 2001). 88 The Zung Self-rating Depression Scale – SDS (Zung and Durham 1965), is another common self-report measure. This scale was originally developed to assess depression in people with a primary diagnosis of depressive disorders. The SDS has 20-items, including somatic and organic symptoms of depression. The items are ranked from 1 to 4 associated with the words ’little or none of the time”, “some of the time”, “a good part of the time” and “most of the time”. Turner and Romano (1984) reported that in a sample of chronic pain patients the SDS has shown good concurrent validity, sensitivity and specificity when compared to BDI. Recent findings suggest that in a sample of chronic pain patients, the Zung’s somatic items were preferentially endorsed when compared to non-somatic items (Taylor et al. 2005). This feature may cause an inflated score on depression in this population when measured by SDS. In the same study, the SDS showed poor sensitivity and specificity to discriminate somatic symptoms of depression from anxiety/stress items in chronic pain patients, as well as in another clinical population and in a sample of general population. More recently, the Depression, Anxiety and Stress Scale – DASS (Lovibond and Lovibond 1993) was developed aiming to provide a more specific measure of depression, anxiety and stress with a lower intercorrelation between these factors, and without including somatic items. The DASS has 3 scales (depression, anxiety and stress) and 42-items ranging from 0 to 3. The Depression scale has 14 items; none of them reflect somatic symptoms. This scale is characterised mainly by loss of self-esteem and incentive, indeed some symptoms of depression that 89 traditionally appear in other scales are not part of DASS – Depression scale, because they do not seem to be specific to depression (Lovibond and Lovibond 1995). It has been reported that the DASS has good psychometric properties and high to moderate correlation with other similar measures. The DASS-depression scale has a moderately high correlation (0.74) with the BDI (Lovibond and Lovibond 1995). Although the BDI has a number of somatic symptoms of depression among its 21 items, 13 reflect cognitive symptoms of depression, thus a moderate-high rather than a high correlation between these two measures is to be expected. The DASS has a high internal consistency, as well as the depression scale (0.91). Furthermore, there is a minimal construct overlap in this measure (Lovibond and Lovibond 1995). Results from Taylor et al. (2005) have also reported a high internal consistency for all the scales (0.96 for the depression scale) with a chronic pain sample, as well as sensitivity and specificity properties of the DASS scales among different groups (clinical patients, chronic pain patients and general population). Recently a short version of the DASS has been developed reporting adequate psychometric properties for this version as well, which confirms the validity and reliability of the original version (Henry and Crawford 2005). 90 2.3.3 Comments As described previously, depression is frequently associated with chronic pain, and it is recognised that depression plays an important role in disability and adjustment to chronic pain, thus its diagnosis, assessment and treatment are important. Although findings are not robust and consistent about the contribution of depression to disability, there is evidence suggesting that depression contribute to suffering and poor outcomes. However, as noted, there is concern that in addition to questions about the models of depression on which these older scales are based, the assessment of depression in medical populations using scales that contain somatic items of depression may lead to bias in their findings. It has also been recognised that the three general depression measures described previously have strong psychometric properties, as well as disadvantages and advantages among them (Bradley and McKendree-Smith 2001). However their use may not be appropriated to chronic pain patients. Among the no-somatic measures of depression, each of these measures have been well-supported (Bejlland et al. 2002; Henry and Crawford 2005; Pincus et al. 2004), as has their use in chronic pain populations (Pincus et al. 2004; Taylor et al. 2005). Of these scales only the HADS has been validated in a Brazilian (non-pain) population (Botega et al. 1995), but none have been validated in a Brazilian chronic pain patient population. 91 Although there is no consensus in the literature about this matter, using a measure to assess depression that does not have somatic items as a part of its construct has been widely encouraged. Based on the literature reviewed, not having somatic items as a part of it appears to be the greatest advantage of the DASS – depression scale among the other measures. Furthermore, the psychometric properties of the depression scale of DASS are as psychometrically sound as the other measures, indeed with stronger psychometric properties in a few aspects. Considering these aspects, its brevity and the fact that the DASS is one of the measures used in the Pain Management and Research Centre – PMRC to assess depression, the DASS- Depression scale is the chosen measure for this series of studies. This will also permit a comparison of the results obtained in a Brazilian population with chronic pain with the population at the PMRC. 92 2.4 Cultural Factors and Pain It has long been thought that ethnicity has an impact on the course of illness and treatment (Dimsdale 2000). These assumptions may be extended to chronic pain to some degree. In the early 1950s Zborowski (1952) suggested that sociocultural factors may affect the psychophysiological processes of pain perception and response to it in different ways. He pointed out that every culture has attitudes towards pain, but their meanings and manifestations differ significantly. Zborowski’s work provided an important early stimulus to studies of cultural factors in pain; subsequent research has broadened understanding of the topic. This section will describe the main models of culture and pain, as well as relevant evidence in this area. 2.4.1 Contribution of Cultural Factors to Chronic Pain Despite Zborowski’s (1952) earlier contribution, Bates (1987), Edwards and Fillingim (1999) pointed out that there were several methodological weaknesses in these early findings. Recent investigation has lead to a better comprehension of the role and interaction of physical factors and learning processes with cultural, ethnic and racial factors in illness and chronic pain. Human beings have basically similar neurophysiological systems, thus differences in pain perception and response cannot be explained 93 only by neurophysiological differences among races (Bates 1987). Rey (1998) also shared this assumption suggesting that nociception reflects anatomy and physiology, but cultural and social factors are the foundation for the expression and treatment of pain. The concept of race, ethnicity and acculturation are central to this discussion, and will be briefly defined. Race may be defined as a group of people united or classified together on the basis of common history, common descent and ancestry or hereditary and genetic features. The concept of ethnicity focuses on the distinction between groups of people based on behaviour and culture, group membership, as well as biology and physical characteristics (Edwards et al. 2001a). Acculturation refers to the process to which migrants go through involving a change of knowledge, cultural beliefs, values, and practices towards those of the host society (Helman 1996). Frequently, people that belong to a specific ethnicity are from the same race, but beside this, they may also share similar habits, norms, characteristics, behaviours, culture, social and psychological nature unless they have been through an acculturation process. According to Fallo (1998), the level of acculturation may be an important intervenient variable between ethnicity and variables such as response to pain, attitudes and beliefs. However, this discussion is not the scope of this review. In the last two decades at least three models have attempted to describe the role of culture in chronic pain. Bates (1987) proposed a biocultural model to explain some issues regarding pain and ethnicity 94 which integrates aspects of the Gate Control Theory, social learning theories and social comparison processes. According to this model, the degree to which sensory transmission increases or decreases is determined by A beta, A delta and C fibres and also by descending influences from the brain. These descending influences are mediated by cognitive or higher brain processes, which can have an important influence on the pain process. Bates suggested "There is evidence that social learning is instrumental in the development of meanings and attitudes toward pain. Learned values and attitudes affect one's attention to painful stimuli and one's memories of prior pain experiences. Therefore, it is likely that cultural group experiences influence the physiological processes responsible for pain threshold and perception of pain severity, as well as pain response" (1987, p. 48). Skevington (1995) proposed a four-stage model to explain the influence of social factors implicated in the generation and maintenance of chronic pain and illness, which shares some features with Bates’ model. The first level regards the individual behaviours and includes perceived bodily sensations, perceived severity of symptoms, lifetime personal and social schemata, lifetime personal and social emotions, lifetime personal representations and personal motivation, which are all in some degree influenced by social processes (e.g. beliefs and social representations). In the second level are interpersonal behaviours, which include thoughts, emotions, motives and behaviours resulting from 95 relationships among those suffering and family members, friends and health professionals. The third level consists of group and inter-group behaviour, “where the identity of the pain sufferer is formed and maintained through social categorisation, social comparisons and identification process” (1995, p. 294). The fourth level concerns higher order factors affecting social-psychological processing, and includes health culture, health history and health ideologies among other macrocomponents. Although there is a dynamic relationship between all levels, Skevington (1995) suggested that there is a descending influence from the fourth and superior stages on all the processes on the lower levels. Wade et al. (1996) also proposed a four-stage model of pain processing, which can contribute to understanding racial/ethnic differences in chronic pain. According to this model, there is an initial sensory-discriminative stage related to the perceived intensity of pain sensation, the second stage is related to the individual immediate affective response to stimuli and involves some cognitive processes. The third stage involves long term cognitive processes related with individual's meaning implications, and the final stage is the behavioural expression of pain. According to this model race/ethnicity has little impact on pain sensation the initial sensory-discriminative stage, but a larger impact on the next two cognitive stages and on the fourth stage, which involves behavioural expressions of pain. According to this model, social and cultural factors might act as moderators of the noxious stimuli in all the pain stages. 96 In general, all these models propose that social comparison and social learning processes within ethno-cultural group situations influence attitudes toward pain, attention to pain stimuli or sensation and cognitive control. Prior pain experiences also participate in this process. Based on these models it could be assumed that attitudes, beliefs, attribution, expectations and assumptions are shaped socially and that culture has an important role in pain perception and response to pain. Several findings support the influence of social and cultural factors in response to pain, although with a variety of results. Zatzick and Dimsdale (1990) reviewed thirteen studies related to response to laboratory and induced pain. They found a great diversity of ethnic groups, pain induced methods and outcomes, and suggested that there appears to be no racial/ethnic differences in the ability to discriminate noxious stimuli. Woodrow et al. (1972) studied 4000 subjects and found that White Americans showed higher pain tolerance than African Americans, and Asian Americans showed lower tolerance than African Americans. Their results also suggest that this difference is more marked in men than in women. Chapman and Jones (1994) reported that White Americans descended from Northern Europeans reported higher thermal pain thresholds and tolerance than African Americans. Wash et al. (1989) also suggest that Saxons have a greater pain tolerance (measured by a cold pressor test) than non Anglo-Saxons (Hispanic and African Americans) across sexes and age. Sheffield et al. (2000) reported that African Americans rated a noxious stimuli as more unpleasant and showed a tendency to rate it as more intense than White 97 Americans, women showed a tendency to rate stimuli as more unpleasant and more intense than men. In addition, systolic blood pressure was inversely related to pain intensity. After statistically adjusting for systolic blood pressure, sex differences in pain unpleasantness were reduced, sex differences in pain intensity were abolished but race differences were unaltered. Although these findings are not robust, yielding mixed evidence, there is no evidence about how precise was the racial grouping and acculturation in the studies involved. Taken together, these studies suggest that ethnic membership (e.g. African Americans) and female gender are associated with greater reported pain and greater pain behaviour display when compared to other groups. Other studies (Sheffield et al., 2000; Edwards et al, 2001a) have found different disease distribution among specific chronic pain diseases related to race, including angina and ischaemic pain among others. These results suggest that there is some specificity involving disease prevalence and race, which is important to note but which may not be important to the discussion regarding the influence of ethnicity on chronic pain. It seems that one of the main issues in this area regards the mechanisms that mediate or moderate these differences and how they may affect response to chronic pain. Results of a study conducted by Honeyman and Jacobs (1996) with a group of aboriginal people in Central Australia reported a pain prevalence range varying from 30 to 50% which is higher than most studies, but not all studies in other countries. However, no illness and 98 pain behaviours were observed as defined in western models (e.g. guarding). These authors also described that the community experience with pain starts in early childhood through spiritual practices, and that they have some beliefs and laws about displaying or complaining about pain. Displaying pain behaviour is not encouraged and there is a strong community expectation about tolerating pain. Thus, pain behaviour and illness are not reinforced and people appear to learn to cope with or accept pain. Lofvander and Furhoff (1996) compared Swedish and Greek patients with back pain and found several differences between these two groups. The Greeks in that study displayed more pain behaviours, use more passive coping strategies and catastrophise more when compared to the Swedish sample. Fallo (1998) also found significant differences in coping strategies among different cultures in South Australia. His findings suggest that Iraq and Central American subjects seemed to use more passive coping strategies than Italians and Chinese subjects. The Iraq group reported more use of praying and displayed more pain behaviours than other groups. Italians and Chinese appeared to attempt to ignore pain. The Italians also used more active coping strategies than the Central American group. Attitudes towards pain among groups were also related to different preferences for coping strategies (e.g. Chinese preferred massage when compared to other groups). How well these findings are generalisable to people of similar ethnicities and other similar situations remains to be shown. 99 Tan et al. (2005) found that Black American patients reported lower perceived control over pain, more external pain-coping strategies, more solicitous beliefs about pain, higher levels of depression and disability, even after controlling for pain intensity. Edwards et al. (2005) compared African, Hispanic and White Americans and found no significant differences in measures of pain, depression, psychopathology and disability, but some differences in coping strategies, with Black and Hispanic Americans reporting higher scores on hoping and praying than White Americans. Hastie et al. (2004) also found significant main effects of ethnicity on four out of seven CSQ-R scales. Results from Bates et al. (1993) study illustrate these differences well. Their findings suggest that pain intensity variations may be affected by differences in attitudes, beliefs and emotional and psychological states associated with different ethnic groups. These authors also found that different ethnic groups have different beliefs about appropriate emotional expression responses to pain. Furthermore the locus of control also appears to differ among groups and play an important role in pain intensity perception. Bates and colleagues (1993) pointed out that culturally acquired patterns might lead to different pain perception but also to different styles of reporting pain. Differences in pain perception can be explained partially taking into account physiological, psychological and ethno cultural factors. Indeed, some research has suggested that anxiety, personality, education level, family history, attentional variables and peripheral mechanisms of nociception do not account for differences between different ethnic 100 groups (Edwards and Fillingim 1999; Lasch 2002; Zatzick and Dimsdale 1990). Other important variables such as beliefs, attitudes, coping strategies, social learning and affect may influence stimuli response, and it appears that these variables vary widely across cultures (Moore and Brodsgaard 1999). It seems that there is also an affective component in cultural bias in pain perception. These differences seem to be smaller in pain threshold measurement, which is a sensory-discriminative dimension of pain (Moore and Brodsgaard 1999). On the other hand, pain tolerance may be related strongly with the affective-motivational dimension (Edwards et al. 1999). It can be hypothesised that different ethnicities may differ more in the affective dimension (e.g. overt pain behaviour) rather than in the sensory processing of noxious stimuli. There is evidence suggesting an association between emotions and pain behaviour. Edwards et al. (2001b) examined responses among different ethnic groups related to pain intensity (how strong the pain feels) and pain unpleasantness reports (how unpleasant the pain is). No differences were found on thermal threshold but differences were found in pain tolerance, with White Americans demonstrating a greater pain tolerance over African Americans. Another study conducted by Riley et al. (2002) found higher levels of pain unpleasantness, emotional responses to pain, and pain behaviour among African Americans when compared to White Americans, but no difference in the degree of pain intensity. Other research also suggested higher levels of depression, post-traumatic stress disorder and sleep disturbance in Black Americans with chronic 101 pain than White Americans (Green et al. 2003). Studies using the MPQ found no ethnic differences in measuring pain sensation but found differences in the affective scales of this instrument. (Lipton and Marbach 1984) These findings reinforce the role of affect on cultural differences and its contribution to responses to pain. More recently, differences in the use of the health system and other outcomes among ethnic groups have been investigated. Lasch (2002) reviewed several studies and found that ethnicity membership may affect provision of medication. Ethnic minority groups also appear to have less access to health services (Nguyen et al. 2005). In general, there is a degree of consensus about the importance of ethnicity in pain perception and response to it. As noted earlier, the models proposed by Bates (1987), Skevington (1995) and Wade et al. (1996) share several features in common and provide support to understand the role of social and cultural factors in shaping perception and response to pain. Evidence on ethnic differences in pain perception and responses to it, have confirmed these theoretical models. However, Edwards et al. (2001a), Edwards et al. (2005) and Riley et al. (2002) have suggested that methodological aspects should be taken into account and recommended caution in the interpretation and generalisation of these findings. Problems such as interference of the experimenter’s ethnicity on results, and controlling for a number of confounding variables (e.g. acculturation, sampling) could affect results significantly. 102 Skevington (1995) pointed that some types of social research have led to patient labelling and categorising instead of understanding the process by which these differences arise. Broadly, the bulk of available findings support the evidence that there are different responses to pain among various ethnic groups. As mentioned by Zatzick and Dimsdale (1990) it seems that it is easier to identify ethnic differences than to explain those differences. 2.4.2. Comments Despite methodological problems and lack of conclusive evidence, it seems that different ethnic groups have only a small variance in pain perception, but more important than this, they seem to respond differently to pain. Mechanisms involved in the relationship between culture and pain are evidently complex and caution is needed in interpreting findings, especially in avoiding confusing description with explanation. Nevertheless, findings described previously have potential clinical importance. Ethnicity has been considered an important variable to be taken into account in the process of diagnosis and treatment, as well as in the development of more specific measures for different ethnicities. From a clinical perspective, ethnicity seems to affect perception and responses to pain, and seems to moderate coping strategies, relationship with health professionals and even seeking health care. 103 However, it should be considered that evidence for this is scarce in this field. To shed more light on the possible role of ethnic differences, ethnic features should be taken into account when considering chronic pain models or treatments developed in different cultures. 104 2.5 Psychological Assessment In the previous sections the role of psychosocial factors in chronic pain has been described. Due to the role of psychosocial factors in chronic pain, psychological assessment of chronic pain patients has been recognised as an essential part of the diagnostic process. Turner and Romano (1999) have argued that defining and assessing these factors is critical to designing effective treatment for people with chronic pain. 2.5.1 Psychological Assessment: Indications and Aims In most instances, a psychological evaluation of people with chronic pain does not intend to establish a diagnosis of the cause of pain. Indeed, psychological evaluation is usually a part of a multidimensional assessment, which aims to understand the role of several factors in the development, modulation and maintenance of chronic pain. According to Tollison and Hinnant (1996), although an assessment is frequently requested when symptoms are greater than would be expected from the physical examination, there are other better indications to request a psychological assessment. A psychological assessment is indicated when pain interferes significantly with the patients’ ability to perform normal activities in many spheres (e.g. social life, work), when there are signs of psychological distress, excessive use of medications or alcohol, and repeated/extensive use of the health care 105 system (Tollison and Hinnant, 1996). A psychological evaluation may also be indicated when patients are moderately or severely disabled. To Turner and Romano (1999), a psychological assessment is useful to identify specific psychological and behavioural factors involved in pain, suffering and disability and poor response to treatment. According to Gamsa (1994) the aims of a psychological assessment are to determine: whether psychological factors play a role in precipitating, exacerbating or perpetuating pain, to assess whether psychological disturbance is a cause or an effect of chronic pain, to determine whether, and which type of intervention is indicated, and to establish rapport with the patient, offer support and answer questions. To Turk and Okifuji (2003) a psychological assessment of chronic pain patients intends to establish a diagnosis, decision making and treatment planning, evaluation of change in symptoms, impact of treatment, prediction of response to treatment and program evaluation. Different theoretical approaches could lead to different aims for a psychological assessment of chronic pain patients. For example: from a Behavioural perspective it is essential to evaluate pain behaviour reinforcement, while in a Gestalt approach to understand how chronic pain is integrated to patients identity may be a key issue in the assessment. Despite some divergences, there is a degree of consensus that the main aim of psychological assessment is to detect cognitive, affective and behavioural factors that may be contributing to pain, disability and distress to assist decision-making and guidance in treatment (Gamsa, 106 1994; Tollison and Hinnant, 1996; Turk and Okifuji, 2003; Turner and Romano, 1999). 2.5.2 Psychological Assessment Methods Pain is a subjective and multidimensional experience. Therefore, by its nature, most pain dimensions can only be assessed indirectly (Turk and Melzack, 2001). The measurement of pain dimensions is fundamental for the comprehension of pain mechanisms, as well as for the evaluation of methods to treat it. The most common methods to assess chronic pain patients' experience regarding psychosocial interviews, psychological testing aspects with are semi-structured self-report questionnaire, behavioural analysis or observation, and health measures. This literature review does not intend to explore all types of assessment, but each method will be briefly described. Semi-structured interview is the most common method to assess cognitive and affective aspects of the patient. Broadly, an interview should cover several dimensions of pain: pain intensity and description, aggravating factors, relevant medical history, past and current treatment, history of drug and alcohol use, history of psychiatric disturbance or emotional trauma, emotional and cognitive factors, disability, working and compensation issues, sleep and daily activities, social aspects and motivational factors (Jamison 2004). There are other topics that may be included in an assessment of people with chronic pain, 107 according to the setting, patient characteristics, types of intervention and theoretical models. Interviews are a useful method of assessing patients’ history and subjective aspects, as well as to check and integrate information. They are a rich source of information, are flexible and adaptable to the needs of the patient and may assess multiple pain domains at the same time (Newton-John 2003). However, they are time consuming; may have excessive bias due to interviewer interpretation, lack of objectivity and difficulty of comparing obtained information with other pain patients (Sardá and Angelotti 2004). Behavioural analysis and observation is a further step in order not to rely only on subjective information. In general, behavioural analysis intends to observe manifestation of how patients respond to pain, which may include posturing, limping, grimacing and other pain behaviours, as well as use of medication. One of its main goals is to identify what factors might be perpetuating pain behaviour (Jamison, 2004). From some perspectives this method of assessment is essential, as it may provide a direct measure of functioning and a comprehension reinforcing environmental factors, providing important information for targeted interventions. However, there is some criticism of behavioural analysis, due to the excessively artificial nature of this method, which may produce unreliable information, as well as lack of standardisation (Asghari and Nicholas, 2001). 108 Assessment of health care use has been a recent measure to assess chronicity and disability. This is aimed at quantifying the use of the health care system, number of sick days, use of pain-related medications, among other aspects (Newton-John, 2003). As health care use is a direct measure, this form of evaluation is promising, and may contribute as an outcome measurement of chronicity and disability. Psychological measures have been widely used in the assessment of chronic pain. There is a wide range of self-report questionnaires to evaluate cognitive, affective, personality and environmental factors. Psychological tests are usually easy to administer and to score. Beside this, test results allow clinicians and researchers to make comparisons among different individuals, as well as various populations. Furthermore, test scores facilitate longitudinal follow-up and research, as well as a quantitative evaluation of interventions (Jensen, 2003; Turk and Melzack, 2001). On the other hand, there is some criticism regarding their potential for response bias and for not being suitable for all patients, specially people with low educational levels, children, aged and handicapped (Newton-John, 2003). Another main criticism of psychometric measures is due to its psychometric properties. Some tests frequently used in the psychological assessment of chronic pain patients were not developed for this population, which makes these measures very unreliable and not valid (Jamison 2004; McDowell and Newell 1996). Beside this, construct validity, test-retest reliability and sample bias are other problematic features of several psychological measures (Jensen, 2001). Once it is 109 guaranteed that the chosen psychological measures have strong psychometric properties, it can be an important device in the assessment of patients with chronic pain. According to Grenn (1985), methods for data collection should meet a few criteria: i. Provide information about cognitive, behavioural, emotional and socio-environmental factors of the target problem; ii. Be quantifiable to be treated statistically; iii. Be sensitive to detect even small changes in the target problem; and iv. Have adequate psychometric properties; specially reliability and validity. These are important criteria to be kept in mind to ensure and enhance the quality of psychological assessment and thus provide a better foundation for the comprehension of chronic pain, other aspects could be observed. The evidence presented suggests that a few principles should also be observed regarding chronic pain patients’ assessment. Firstly, psychological assessment of people with chronic pain must always be multidimensional due to the characteristics of this phenomenon. Secondly, assessment methods should be determined by the assessment goals. Third, assessment methods should be complementary (e.g. interviews and psychological tests). Fourth, it must be kept in mind that interventions are largely dependent upon the accuracy and sensitivity of the evaluation methods. Thus, selecting an appropriate, reliable and valid method is determinant to achieving an adequate comprehension of the aspects involved in chronic pain, as well as to design efficacious interventions. 110 Evidence suggests that psychological factors play an important role in disability and emotional adjustment; therefore they should be included in the evaluation of chronic pain patients. Furthermore it is recognised that psychological testing is a useful method as a part of the psychological assessment. However, there is a lack of psychological measures developed for the assessment of chronic pain patients in Brazil (Sardá et al. 2005). 111 2.6. Final Comments Based on the evidence gathered this series of studies is based on the hypothesis that the biopsychosocial perspective of pain is applicable to a Brazilian chronic pain population. This series of studies aim to: 1) Validate a number of measures on cognitive and affective domains of chronic pain. 2) Test whether the relationships between the validated measures are consistent with current biopsychosocial models of pain. 3) To compare two chronic pain samples from different cultures and countries on the same measures, specifically a Brazilian and an Australian sample. Providing satisfactory validation for the proposed measures will collaborate to enhance resources regarding psychological measures available to the Brazilian chronic pain population. This series of studies may also help to improve research possibilities, especially the ability to collaborate with research in other countries. Furthermore, this series of studies may confirm the evidence published in the international literature. 112 CHAPTER THREE Study One: Psychometric Properties of Measures of Disability, Pain SelfEfficacy Beliefs, Catastrophising, Acceptance and Depression in a Brazilian Population with Chronic Pain. 3.1 Introduction In the previous chapters chronic pain definitions and pain prevalence were described. Overall, it was concluded that although there is still some divergence on the definition of chronic pain, there is strong evidence suggesting that there is a high prevalence of chronic pain among most of societies and that chronic pain often has a high psychosocial and economic impact. However, it was also noted that there is considerable variance in the impact of chronic pain between individuals. Biopsychosocial perspectives attempt to account for this variance by exploring the role of psychosocial factors interacting with biological factors in the development and maintenance of chronic pain and its impact on a person’s life. Evidence was found that some psychological factors, such as catastrophising, self-efficacy, acceptance and depression may mediate the relationship between chronic pain, disability and emotional adjustment (e.g. Linton, 2000; Keefe et al., 2004). 113 Based on this evidence, the assessment of psychological factors has been recognised as an integral part of the assessment of chronic pain patients. There are different methods used to conduct a psychological assessment. Standardised psychological measures are generally widely recommended for this purpose and are the most commonly used assessment method. There are literally dozens of different measures used to assess pain dimensions; however this literature review identified only sixteen instruments currently being used for this purpose in Brazil (Sardá et al. 2005). Among those instruments, only about half have been validated in a Brazilian chronic pain population. These are: McGill Pain Questionnaire – MPQ (Pimenta and Teixeira 1997), the Beck Depression Inventory – BDI (Cunha 2001), the Survey of Pain Attitude – SOPA (Pimenta and Cruz 2006), the Short Form Health Survey (SF-36) (Ciconelli et al. 1998), the Roland and Morris Disability Questionnaire – RMDQ (Nusbaum et al. 2001), the Coping Strategies Questionnaire – CSQ (Portnoi et al. 2005), the Chronic Pain Self-Efficacy Scale – CPSS (Salvetti and Pimenta 2005) and Numeric or Verbal Pain Rating Scales. However, relatively few studies have been reported with these validated measures in Brazil. Not surprisingly, a number of other dimensions or constructs that seem to be important in relation to pain are not assessed by these instruments. For example, the role of acceptance, which is a novel concept in the chronic pain field, has become the focus of systematic research only recently. To date, the only studies examining this construct 114 have been from North America (McCracken 1999), Europe (McCracken et al. 2004b) and recently in Australia (Nicholas and Asghari 2006). The extent to which that research is applicable to chronic patients in Brazil is presently unknown. Furthermore, the construct validity of some of the above measures to chronic pain patients is questionable (e.g. the presence of somatic items on the BDI may cause a bias in the assessment of depression in patients with chronic pain) (Pincus and Williams 1999). In addition, while there has been some Brazilian research on the constructs of catastrophising (Portnoi et al. 2005) and self-efficacy (Salvetti and Pimenta 2005), this has been limited to a few studies and clearly requires further exploration. It might also be worthwhile to investigate these constructs with different measures to test the generalisation of the earlier findings. Different measures of apparently the same construct could result in different findings, providing different perspectives. For example, in relation to self-efficacy measurement, the Chronic Pain Self-Efficacy Scale (Anderson et al., 1995) does not ask or emphasise that the patient should consider the fact of being in pain when answering about his/her self-confidence to execute some tasks. This can be contrasted with another pain self-efficacy measure, the Pain SelfEfficacy Questionnaire – PSEQ (Nicholas 1989; Nicholas 2007), which does take pain into account when considering the confidence in performing tasks. In both cases, comparisons of studies that apparently investigate the same construct will clarify the importance of these constructs for 115 disability, as well as, the validity and reliability of these measures to a Brazilian population. In general, due to the lack of studies with psychological measures for the Brazilian population suffering from chronic pain, further studies are needed to test hypotheses generated by previous research. To confirm theoretical psychological models of chronic pain generated in other countries, and to provide a wider range of validated measures. This study should contribute to improve the assessment and treatment of Brazilian chronic pain patients. Accordingly, the purpose of this study is to examine the validity and reliability of a range of psychological instruments internationally used on a Brazilian population with chronic pain. Where no Portuguese translation of the instruments was available, the first task was to develop acceptable translated versions. 116 3.2 Method 3.2.1 Subjects Over a period of four months (from March to June 2005), data were collected from 348 subjects with chronic pain attending pain clinics or similar facilities in the public and private sector. The subjects should be considered to represent a convenience sample of patients attending pain clinics, selected on the basis of specified criteria. Thirty seven subjects (11%) were excluded after applying inclusion and exclusion criteria. Data presented on Results are from 311 subjects, which was the final sample. 3.2.2 Inclusion criteria • Patients having chronic pain for a period of more than 3 months over the past 6 months on most of the days; • Age superior to 18 and inferior to 80; • Having four or more years of formal education; • Willing to participate in the study and spend 40 minutes completing questionnaires. 117 3.2.3 Exclusion criteria • Having cancer pain2; • Having a diagnosis of a major psychiatric disorder, such as psychoses or dementia; • Questionnaires with more than 10% of missing items. 3.2.4 Procedure In general our procedure consisted of two parts, (i) translation (into Portuguese) and adaptation of measures and (ii) data collection. The procedure for data collection is described below, while the procedure regarding the translation of the measures is described in the measure section. Brazilian chronic pain patients attending pain institutions willing to participate in this study were referred by the main physician to the researcher who provided necessary explanation for the patients, a written consent form and the measures to be completed. Questionnaires took approximately 40 minutes to be completed, after that they were checked by the researcher to ensure there were no missing items. Medical/clinical data then were collected from the patients’ medical files. 2 In comparison to those with chronic non-cancer pain, there are often other issues involved with cancer pain (e.g. survival, aversive treatments, beliefs) which may influence the assessment of variables like depression. To avoid these issues these patients were excluded from the studied sample. 118 Data were collected in nine different institutions in the South and South-East regions of Brazil (listed in Appendix I). All the ethical procedures were observed, including information about the purpose of the research and patients’ rights3. No change in treatment occurred. 3.2.5 Measures In previous sections a number of measures of disability, selfefficacy, depression, acceptance and catastrophising have been described and examined. Since the psychometric properties of these measures were good, other criteria influenced scale selection. The data collected in a Brazilian population in this study will be compared with a chronic pain patient sample attending the Pain Management and Research Centre (PMRC) - Royal North Shore Hospital (Study 3). Thus, since psychometric properties were adequate, being used at the Pain Management and Research Centre was an important consideration in scale selection. All the measures used in this studies have not been previously validated to the Brazilian population, except the back pain version of the Roland and Morris Disability Questionnaire (Nusbaum et al. 2001). The translations of the measures were based on a back-translation method (Guillemin et al. 1993) which involved reviewing, translating and 3 The consent form is in appendix II, and the ethic committee approval on appendix III. 119 adapting the measures from English into Portuguese (and back to English again). A preliminary cross-cultural adaptation was necessary because some of the words used to express feelings or situations would not make sense or would lose their meaning if they were translated literally to Portuguese (e.g. “I can’t go on any more” was translated as “Eu não consigo mais continuar”). After the measures were translated and adapted to Portuguese by the researcher, they were given to three Brazilian examiners who are involved with health psychology and who speak English as their second language to be translated independently back to English. After that, the three translations were compared and no important divergence was found except for a few minor semantic differences (i.e. 85% of the items were translated in the same way by the reviewers) that were resolved by discussion between the researcher and reviewers. Once there was agreement between the researcher and reviewers, that the initial translations were satisfactory, a final back translation to Portuguese was carried out. Overall, the main problems in translation related to a few semantic issues. For example, in some sentences instead of using “my pain” only “pain” was used, in other sentences the order of words were changed to make the grammar more appropriate in Portuguese. Apart from these minor changes, the Portuguese versions of the tests were kept as close as possible to the original English version without compromising its comprehension and being adequate to a Portuguese reader. 120 The last back translation was then compared with the original translation done by the researcher. A high rate of concordance (i.e. in 90% of the items there were no major changes) occurred between the measures. All the psychometric properties, characteristics, scoring procedures and relevant evidence of the chosen measures have already been described in the literature review section. Thus the following measures used in this series of studies and listed bellow will not be described again. The Roland and Morris Disability Questionnaire – RMDQ (Roland and Morris 1983), the Pain Self-Efficacy Questionnaire - PSEQ (Nicholas 1989), the PRSS - Catastrophising Scale (Flor et al. 1993), the Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire - CPAQ (McCracken et al. 2004b), and the DASS - Depression scale (Lovibond and Lovibond 1993), a socio demographic and a clinical inventory were used to collect data on these dimensions. Socio demographic questionnaire The socio-demographic questionnaire collected information regarding: age, gender, marital status, level of education, profession and working status. 121 Clinical Inventory The clinical inventory had question regarding: pain site, clinical diagnosis, pain intensity and duration (assessed by a numerical and verbal rating scale), medication usage, type of intervention, and other major health problems. 3.3 Aims of study The major aim of this study was to assess the psychometric properties of a pain-related disability, depression, pain-related selfefficacy beliefs, catastrophising and acceptance questionaries, especially to test the validity and reliability of these measures in a Brazilian chronic pain population. 3.4 Statistical analyses A number of statistical procedures were conducted to test the psychometric properties of these measures, including: i. Descriptive statistics (means, standard deviations, medians, ranges); ii. Reliability; iii. Validity. Reliability was assessed using split-half reliability (Pearson correlation) and internal consistency (Cronbach's alpha) calculations. Validity was assessed by analysis of construct and criterion validity. Construct validity was examined using convergent validity (item- 122 scale correlations) (Hammond 2000), discriminant validity (correlation between different scales or measures) and principal components analysis – PCA (items factors loading, communalities). Criterion validity was assessed comparing mean differences between groups on education level and working status (Anastasi and Urbina 1997). The analysis of criterion validity was based on the hypothesis that some differences between groups should occur on some scales (e.g. work status and scores on disability), but not between some other variables (e.g. educational level and depression). All the analyses were conducted using the SPSS for Windows package, version 14.0. 123 3.5 Results Although initially there were 348 subjects in the sample, 37 subjects did not meet the inclusion and exclusion criteria. Data presented below is from the final sample of 311 subjects. However to evaluate the quality of the data and search for any significant differences between the total sample and the final sample, the two groups were compared with each other. There were no significant differences between the two groups regarding gender, age, level of education, employment status, pain site and mean scores in all the measures (p≤ 0.05). In the excluded subjects group the pattern of missing items in the measures were similar to the final sample. However, most of the excluded questionnaires came from one pain clinic in southern Brazil where the procedure for data collection was different than the one used in the other institutions. In this clinic the questionnaires were given to the subjects to be completed at home and returned on the patients’ second appointment. In another pain clinic located in the South East region of Brazil that also treats chronic pain patients with cancer, questionnaires completed by these patients were excluded from the final sample. Due to the fact that the researcher did not collect all the data (i.e. in 3 pain centres data was collected by research assistant) it is not possible to specify how many patients were approached, but overall most of the patients who were approached and met the inclusion criteria were willing to participate in the study. 124 Table 3.1 Socio-demographic characteristics of the study sample Socio-Demographic Characteristics No. (%) Age group (years) 18 - 30 39 (12.5) 31 - 42 61 (19.6) 43 - 54 105 (33.8) 55 - 66 69 (22.2) 67 - 81 37 (11.9) Mean (SD) 48.9 (14.06) Gender Male Female 81 (26.0) 230 (74.0) Marital status Single Married Separated, divorced or widowed 55 (17.7) 200 (64.3) 56 (18.0) Educational Level 4 to 8 years (primary level) 9 to 11 years (secondary level and/or technical) Higher level of education (i.e. university) 101 (32.5) 89 (28.6) 121 (38.9) Employment status (no = 304) Working 157 (51.6) Partially working 22 Unemployed due to pain 125 (41.1) (7.2) Region South 164 (52.7) South-east 147 (47.3) The age distribution of the sample was normal, with a higher concentration in the 43 to 54 years group. There were more females than males (74% to 26%, respectively). Most of the subjects were married, and in the separated group, 39 (12.5%) subjects were separated or divorced and 17 (5.5%) were widowed. The sample’s education level was 125 evenly distributed, with a slightly higher proportion of subjects with tertiary education. Most of the subjects were working, but a substantial minority (41%) was not working due to pain. Data collected in the southern region were gathered in Florianopolis - Santa Catarina in the following institutions: Rheumatology Ward and Acupuncture Ward at the University Hospital - UFSC, IPE-IMTC (Acupuncture Clinic), Nidi-Neuroscience Pain Clinic and Santa Clara Clinic. Data collected from the south-eastern region were gathered in São Paulo city, in the pain clinics of the following institutions: 9 de Julho Hospital, AC Camargo Hospital, State Public Workers Hospital, and at the Fundão University Hospital – UFRJ in Rio de Janeiro state. Most of the data were collected from patients attending public services (n=164, 52.6%). On Table 2 data related to the clinical characteristics of the sample are presented. 126 Table 3.2 Clinical characteristics of the study sample Clinical Characteristics No. (%) Pain site Head, face, neck 36 (11.6) Cervical, shoulders and upper limbs 48 (15.5) Thoracic and abdominal 14 (4.5) Back or spine alone 16 (5.1) Lower limbs 22 (7.1) 5 (1.6) Lower back and lower limbs 30 (9.6) Two or more major sites 140 (45.0) Pelvic, anal, perineal and genital Pain duration From 3 months up to 1 year 28 (12.2) 1 to 3 years 60 (19.3) 3 to 5 years 87 (28.0) 6 to 9 years 39 (12.5) Up to 10 years 87 (28.0) Pain intensity (average during past week according a NRS) 1 to 4 (mild) 70 (22.5) 5 to 6 (moderate) 94 (30.2) 7 to 10 (severe) 147 (47.3) Stress intensity due to pain (average during past week according a NRS) 0 to 4 (mild) 60 (19.3) 5 to 6 (moderate) 64 (20.6) 7 to 10 (severe) 187 (60.1) Undergone treatments Medication 256 (82.4) Conservative (i.e. physiotherapy, massage) 78 (25.0) Invasive (i.e. nerve blocks, radiofrequency, surgery) 81 (26.0) Types of medication Corticoid-steroids + analgesics 20 (6.5) Analgesics + anti-depressive, sedative-hypnotics or anti-convulsants 32 (10.3) Combination of three categories of medication 54 (17.4) Analgesics 74 (23.8) Anti-depressive, sedative-hypnotics or anti-convulsants 76 (24.4) 127 In relation to the pain site, a substantial proportion of subjects (45%) had pain in two or more sites. The next most common pain site was the cervical region/shoulders/upper-limbs (15.5%), followed by pain in the back or lower back region associated or not with irradiation to the lower limbs (14.7%). Pain in the lower limbs (7.1%) was described as a separated category because it usually consisted of pain in the knees and joints and was usually associated with osteoarthritis. Mean pain duration was 4.3 years, but a large number of subjects had pain for over 3 years (68.5%) and 28% of the subjects had had pain for over 10 years. Pain intensity was measured on a numerical rating scale (0 to 10 associated with the words no pain and the most severe pain on the extremities) and categorized as mild pain (1 to 4), moderate pain (5 to 6), and severe pain (7 to 10) (Jensen et al. 2001a). There was a small prevalence of mild pain intensity (22.5) and a large number of subjects (47.3%) reporting severe pain, mean pain intensity was 6. Furthermore, the majority of subjects (60.1%) reported severe stress due to pain (using a 0 to 10 numerical scale to rate stress) (i.e. 1-4, 5-6 and 7-10), mean stress intensity was 6.9 (SD=2.9). The majority of subjects (82.4%) were taking medications for their pain and nearly half reported having had conservative procedures (25%) such as physiotherapy, osteopathy and massage or have done an invasive procedure (26%) (e.g. radiofrequency lesion, nerve blocks). In relation to the type of medication, subjects in the corticoidssteroids + analgesic group could be also using immunosuppressors or quimiotherapics and in general were being treated for arthritis. The 128 second group labelled analgesics + anti-depressive, sedative-hypnotics or anti-convulsants, could be taking a muscle relaxant, a simple analgesic or a compound analgesic associated with a psychoactive medication. The third group was using a combination of three or more medications, usually of analgesics, opioid and psychoactive drugs. The fourth group was taking single muscle relaxant, simple analgesic, compound analgesic, anti-inflammatory or a combination of them, and the fifth group was taking anti-depressive, sedative-hypnotics or anti-convulsants or a combination of them. The most used psychoactive medication was Amitriptyline and Carbamazepine. Prevalence of co-morbidities was also investigated. The most common diseases in this population were hypertension (28%), followed by psychiatric disorders (18%, mainly depression), endocrine dysfunctions (10%) and diabetes (5%). Statistical analyses were run to analyse the distribution of the responses for each measure and a number of other variables (e.g. age, pain intensity). These indicated normal distributions in each case, with skewness and kurtosis values not reaching 1.0 (Tabachnick and Fidell 2001). Furthermore no clear outliers were identified either. Thus, no statistical transformation to normalise the data was necessary. 129 Table 3.3 Descriptive statistics for all the measures Questionnaires Means (SD) Skewness Kurtosis Floor (%) Ceiling (%) Score range Roland and Morris Disability Questionnaire 12.03 (6.21) -0.02 -1.0 1.0 1.9 0-24 Pain Self-Efficacy Questionnaire - PSEQ 34.84 (14.08) -0.28 -0.63 0.3 1.0 0-60 DASS - Depression Scale 14.03 (12.02) 0.79 -0.56 7.7 1.6 0-42 PRSS - Catastrophising Scale 2.38 (1.38) 0.14 -0.94 2.3 4.5 0-5 Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire 60.20 (18.11) -0.30 0.13 0.3 0.6 0-120 Activity Engagement Scale 39.89 (14.07) -0.42 -0.37 0.3 1.0 0-66 Pain Willingness Scale 20.21 (11.08) 0.18 -0.54 2.6 0.3 0-54 Means and standard deviations for the Brazilian versions of each questionnaire were compared with those obtained in the original standardisation excepted for the RMDQ that was compared with a Brazilian standardisation for low back pain patients (Nusbaum, 2001). 130 Tables 3.4 Comparison between mean scores of the Brazilian sample and the original questionnaire Questionnaires t-test of Means (SD) Means (SD) Brazilian Original sample sample 12.03 (6.21) 14.30 (7.47)a 1.62 Pain Self-Efficacy Questionnaire 34.84 (14.08) 23.50 (13.1)b 4.68* DASS - Depression Scale 14.03 (12.02) 15.15 (12.22)c significance Roland Morris Disability Questionnaire Catastrophising Scale 2.38 (1.38) 1.23 2.03 (1.22)d 3.18* Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire 60.20 (18.11) 56.3 (20.4)e 2.32* Activity Engagement Scale 39.89 (14.07) 33.3 (13.2)e 4.25* Pain Willingness Scale 20.21 (11.08) 13.8 (7.9)e 7.91* * Significant mean differences at a expected t is 1.96, p=0.001 a (Nusbaum et al. 2001), b (Nicholas 2007), c (Taylor et al. 2005), d (Flor et al. 1993), e (McCracken 1999) T-tests results indicated a significant difference between the PSEQ mean score (Nicholas 2007) and the PSEQ mean score on the Brazilian sample, the CPAQ Brazilian mean score and its sub-scales mean score were also significantly different when compared to the published standardisation score (McCracken et al., 1999). Mean score on the PRSS - Catastrophising scale Brazilian version was also significantly different when compared to the original version (Flor et al., 1992). In all the measures described above the Brazilian samples had higher mean score when compared with the original sample. 131 On the other hand, mean scores of the RMDQ general version (used in this study) and the preliminary standardisation with back pain patients done in a Brazilian sample (Nusbaum et al., 2001) did not differ. However this validation of the RMDQ for low back pain in the Brazilian population had only 30 subjects. Mean score on the DASS – Depression scale Brazilian version also did not differ significantly from an Australian chronic pain patient sample (Taylor et al. 2005). Reliability characteristics of these translated measures are presented bellow. Table 3.5 Internal consistency and split-half correlation of the measures Questionnaires Number of items Internal Correlation between consistency forms (split-half) Cronbach’s α Pearson coefficient RMDQ 24 0.90 0.82 PSEQ 10 0.90 0.76 DASS - Depression Scale 14 0.96 0.93 9 0.89 0.74 CPAQ 20 0.78 - Activity Engagement Scale 11 0.85 0.73 9 0.76 0.57 PRSS - Catastrophising Scale Pain Willingness Scale All scales had a Cronbach α higher than 0.70, which suggests an adequate degree of internal consistency for research purposes (Jensen 2003). The RMDQ, PSEQ, Depression scale and the Catastrophising scale all had a Cronbach α at a superior level (or nearly 0.90), which indicate 132 an excellent internal consistency, which confirm the reliability of these measures (Nunnaly and Bernstein 1994). According to Jensen (2003) Cronbach α values above 0.90 indicate an adequate degree of internal consistency for clinical purposes as well. Due to the fact that the CPAQ has two scales, split half correlation was run for each scale independently. On the depression scale the first half was composed of items 1, 3, 6, 9, 10, 13 and 14, which is consistent with the Depression scale short form (Henry and Crawford 2005). In both cases when an analysis was run using other split half methods results were similar. For the other questionnaires and scales split-half correlation the researcher choose to use the default mode of the SPSS 14.0 program, which selects the first half of the scale items and compares them with the second half items. Pearson correlation coefficients for the split-half forms of all the questionnaires were in a moderate to high range for most of the measures, except for the Pain Willingness scale split-half correlation (0.57) (Anastasi and Urbina 1997). Although analysis of reliability using internal consistency coefficient have shown an adequate internal consistency for all the instruments, split-half correlation coefficients indicate a lack of consistency for the Pain Willingness scale. To examine this feature and assess the validity of the measures further analysis including Principal Component Analysis (PCA) was run. Construct validity was examined through item-scale correlations, principal component analysis, and correlation between scales. Criterion 133 validity was assessed comparing mean differences between groups on education level and working status. Table 3.6 Item-scale correlation for the CPAQ Items Activity Engagement scale Pain Willingness scale 1 0.69** 0.24 2 0.76** 0.46 3 0.35** - 0.16** 4 - 0.04 0.60** 5 0.53** 0.08 6 0.76** 0.08 7 - 0.10 0.44** 8 0.63** 0.07 9 0.78** 0.13* 10 0.51** 0.10 11 - 0.10 12 0.67** 13 - 0.00 0.65** 14 0.10 0.70** 15 0.66** 0.48** 0.07 0.04 16 0.03 0.64** 17 0.01 0.53** 18 0.11* 0.66** 19 0.61** - 0.12* 20 Activity Engagement scale Pain Willingness scale 0.08 0.51** 1 0.51 0.51 1 Activity Engagement scale: items 1, 2, 3, 5, 6, 8, 9, 10, 12, 15, 19 Pain Willingness scale: items 4, 7, 11, 13, 14, 16, 17, 18, 20 * level of significance:0.05 **level of significance: 0.01 Most correlations between items and the relevant scale were moderate or nearly in this range (Pearson correlation range 0.44 to 0.78 at a p< 0.01), except for item 3 which had a correlation of 0.35 (items 134 were not adjusted). Also a moderate inter-correlation between the scales (0.51) suggests adequate convergent validity of the scales. The next phase of these investigations involved conducting principal component analysis to explore the factor structure of each measure (see Table 7 for the CPAQ results). Factor loadings, communalities (h²), eigenvalue and percentage of variance indicate the existence of a 5 factor solution for the CPAQ. Since correlations among some factors were lower than 0.32 they were submitted to a varimax rotation (Tabachnick and Fidell 2001). This model accounted for 57% of variance and five main factors were identified using the eigenvalue greater than 1 criterion. All items had a moderate to high loading on its factors (range from .50 to .77). The first factor retained seven of the eleven original items reported by McCracken (1999), the second factor had six of the original nine items, while the other seven items loaded to factor 3, 4 and 5, although item three emerged as a single-item factor. Factors 1 and 2 accounted for the higher percentage of variance, while factor 5 (item 3) should be reconsidered. Correlations between factors are presented in the last table. Internal consistency analysis for a 4 factor solution for the CPAQ is presented in Table 8. 135 Table 3.7 PCA of the CPAQ factors loading and communalities for a 5 factor solution Item number and description Factor 1: Activity Engagement (AC) (12) Despite the pain, I am now sticking to a certain course in my life (15) Even when my pain increases, I can still take care of my responsibilities (9) I lead a full life even though I have chronic pain (8) There are many activities I do, even when I fell pain (1) I am getting on with the business of living no matter what my level of pain is (2) My life is going well, even though I have chronic pain (6) Although things have changed, I am living a normal life despite my chronic pain Factor 2: Pain Willingness (PW) (17) I avoid putting myself in situations where my pain might increase (20) I have to struggle to do things when I have pain (13) Keeping my pain level under control takes first priority whenever I'm doing something (4) I would gladly sacrifice important things in my life to control this pain better (14) Before I can make any serious plans, I have to get some control over my pain (18)My worries and fears about what pain will do to me are true Factor 3:Thought Control (7) I need to concentrate on getting rid of my pain (11) My thoughts and feelings about pain must change before I can take important steps in my life (16) I will have better control over my life if I can control my negative thoughts about pain Factor 4:Chronicity (5) It's not necessary for me to control my pain in order to handle my life well (10) Controlling pain is less important than any other goals in my life (19) It's a relief to realise that I don't have to change my pain to get on with my life Factor 5:Accept suffering (3) It's OK to experience pain Eigenvalue Percentage variance CPAQ F1 original subscales F2 F3 F4 F5 h² AC .76 -.28 .08 .04 .07 .60 AC .75 .07 -.13 .04 -.15 .62 AC .73 .05 0.10 .29 .14 .65 AC .72 -.02 .02 .02 -.14 .54 AC .70 .05 -.04 .13 .18 .55 AC .69 .04 .03 .31 .26 .65 AC .64 .06 .03 .42 .20 .63 PW .27 .73 -.07 -.06 -.04 .55 PW -.06 .72 -.02 .19 .24 .62 PW -.00 .60 .30 .02 -.21 .50 PW .06 .57 .16 -.12 -.43 .56 PW .04 .54 .47 .16 -.10 .56 PW .17 .50 .42 -.05 -.04 .47 PW -.13 -.03 .67 .05 -.03 .48 PW .11 .06 .67 -.24 .03 .53 PW .01 .33 .63 .07 -.03 .51 AC .16 .08 .03 .75 .13 .62 AC .28 .10 -.06 .63 -.32 .59 AC .40 -.17 -.10 .54 -.01 .50 AC .25 -.09 -.04 -.04 .77 .68 4.72 3.24 1.35 23.61 16.23 6.75 1.08 5.40 1.01 5.09 136 Table 3.8 Means (SD) and internal consistency for a 4 factor solution Factor description Factor 1 Mean (SD) 27.40 (10.30) Internal Mean inter- consistency item Cronbach’s α correlation 0.87 0.48 Factor 2 11.36 (8.05) 0.75 0.32 Factor 3 8.94 (4.63) 0.50 0.24 Factor 4 8.06 (4.84) 0.58 0.31 Only factors 1 and 2 had a Cronbach α higher than 0.70, which suggests an adequate internal consistency. Although Cronbach α for factor 3 and 4 were in the medium range, it suggests a weak correlation among the items and a low internal consistency (Jensen, 2003). Low mean interitem correlation among the items in all factors, except for factor 1, which has a moderate correlation (0.48), also confirms the results presented earlier regarding the internal consistency of the CPAQ and its scales. In the next tables principal components analysis results from the PSEQ, Catastrophising and Depression scales are presented. 137 Table 3.9 PCA of the PSEQ factors loading and communalities for a 1 factor solution Item number and description F1 h² (8) I can still accomplish most of my goals in life, despite the pain. .83 .69 (1) I can enjoy things, despite the pain. .81 .66 (9) I can have a normal lifestyle, despite the pain. .81 .65 (10) I can gradually become more active, despite the pain. .80 .64 (6) I can still do many of the things I enjoy doing such as hobbies .77 .60 .76 .58 .73 .54 (4) I can cope with my pain in most situations. .72 .52 (3) I can socialise with my friends and family members as often as I .67 .44 .44 .20 or leisure activities, despite the pain. (5) I can do some form of work, despite the pain (work includes housework, paid and unpaid work). (2) I can do most of my household chores (e.g. tiding up, washing dishes, etc.), despite the pain. used to do, despite the pain. (7) I can cope with my pain without medication. Eigenvalue 5.53 Percentage variance Principal components 55.29 analysis using oblique and orthogonal rotation and applying the eigenvalue greater than 1 criterion identified the existence of only one factor. This factor accounts for 55.29% of the total variance, high loadings (range from .72 to .83) in eight out of ten items and median communalities on eight items confirms the existence of only one factor on the Pain Self-Efficacy Questionnaire. 138 Table 3.10 PCA of the Catastrophising Scale factors loading and communalities for a 2 factor solution Item number and description F1 F2 h² (8) I can’t go on any more. .98 -.09 .86 (9) This pain is driving me crazy. .88 -.02 .76 (1) I cannot stand this pain any longer. .86 -.03 .72 (7) This pan is killing me. .76 .14 .73 (3) I need to take some pain medication. .38 .17 .25 (4) This will never end. -.08 .91 .74 (5) I am a hopeless case. .00 .86 .75 (2) No matter what I do my pain doesn’t change. .03 .78 .65 (6) When will it get worse again? .15 .68 .61 Eigenvalue 4.91 1.17 Percentage variance 54.5 12.9 Factor 1 Rumination Factor 2 Helplessness Principal component analysis using oblique and orthogonal rotation and applying the eigenvalue greater than 1 criterion identified the existence of two factors, named rumination and helplessness. A two factor solution accounted for 67.5% of total variance and most loading values were above 0.70 (range .68 to .98), excepted for item 3 (0.38 and h²= .25). Considering its loading, communalities and face value the catastrophising scale appears to have more than one dimension, and 139 probably a 2 factor solution may be appropriated to describe the concept of catastrophising. Table 3.11 Mean (SD) and internal consistency for a 2 factor solution Factor description Mean (SD) Internal Mean inter- consistency item Cronbach’s α correlation Factor 1: Magnification 2.78 (1.52) .86 .55 Factor 2: Helplessness 1.87 (1.05) .85 .58 A Cronbach’s α higher than 0.70 and nearly 0.90 in both factors suggest an excellent internal consistency for this scale. A moderate interitem correlation (above 0.40) and a correlation of 0.59 between factor 1 and 2 also confirms the validity of this measure. 140 Table 3.12 PCA of the Depression Scale with factors loading and communalities for a 1 factor solution Item number and description F1 h² 5. I felt that I had lost interest in just about everything. .87 .76 7. I felt that life wasn’t worthwhile. .87 .75 12. I could see nothing in the future to be hopeful about. .85 .72 11. I felt I was pretty worthless. .84 .70 10. I was unable to become enthusiastic about anything. .83 .69 6. I felt I wasn’t worth much as a person. .82 .68 9. I felt down hearted and blue. .82 .67 3. I felt that I had nothing to look forward. .81 .66 13. I felt that life was meaningless. .80 .64 4. I felt sad and depressed. .80 .64 8. I couldn’t seem to get any enjoyment out of things I did. .79 .62 2. I just couldn’t seem to get going. .77 .59 14. I found it difficult to work up the initiative to do things. .73 .53 1. I couldn’t seem to experience any positive feelings at all. .64 .40 Eigenvalue 9.10 Percentage variance 65.04 Principal component analysis using oblique and orthogonal rotation and applying the eigenvalue greater than 1 criterion identified the existence of only one factor for the depression scale of the DASS. This accounted for 65.05% of total variance. High loadings (range .73 to .87) 141 for all items except for item one (.64) and high communality among items confirms the existence of only one factor on the depression scale. The results of comparisons made between different demographic groups (using 1-way ANOVA) in an attempt to assess the criterion validity of the measures are presented in the next tables. Table 3.13 Comparison of mean score in questionnaires by educational level Questionnaires 4 to 8 years 9 to 11 years Higher level of education of education of education significance mean (SD) mean (SD) mean (SD) F P RMDQ 14.55 (6.31) 12.15 (5.62) 9.85 (5.76) 17.46 0.01 PSEQ 32.94 (14.06) 33.51 (14.46) 37.40 (13.53) 3.36 0.04 Depression Scale 15.80 (12.99) 13.80 (12.44) 12.72 (10.70) 1.83 0.16 2.37 (1.45) 2.50 (1.45) CPAQ 62.17 (16.34) AE Scale PW Scale Catastrophising 2.30 (1.34) Test of 0.56 0.57 57.69 (18.90) 60.39 (18.83) 1.46 0.23 40.15 (12.78) 29.77 (14.95) 39.76 (14.52) 0.26 0.10 21.92 (10.96) 17.98 (10.69) 20.14 (11.28) 3.05 0.05 No significant differences were found between groups on the Depression Scale (DASS), Activity Engagement Scale, Pain Willingness scale, the CPAQ and on the catastrophising Scale. On the Roland Morris Questionnaire the group with higher education had significant lower means when compared with the 9 to 11 years of education group (p=0.001) and with the 4 to 8 years of education (p=0.02). In the Pain Self-Efficacy Questionnaire although some significant differences were 142 initially found, further analysis (Post Hoc analysis using Scheffe to analyse significance) did not reveal any significant differences between groups. Comparison between groups on the basis of work status for each measure is presented below. Tables 3.14 Comparison of mean scores on questionnaires by working status Questionnaires Partially Not working Test of Working due to pain significance mean (SD) Mean (SD) mean (SD) F ρ RMDQ 9.29 (5.78) 12.61 (6.53) 14.78 (5.21) 29.52 0.001 PSEQ 39.70 (13.22) 34.71 (13.70) 29.84 (13.01 17.17 0.001 Depression Scale 11.05 (10.81) 15.38 (13.58) 17.62 (12.09) 9.84 0.001 2.31 (1.32) 2.14 (1.63) 2.65 (1.36) 2.43 0.09 CPAQ 66.23 (15.03) 54.85 (16.85) 52.67(14.48) 21.98 0.001 AE Scale 43.91 (12.91) 35.80 (13.04) 35.73 (14.03) 12.23 0.001 PW Scale 22.29 (10.56) 18.97 (10.38) 16.67 (10.51) 0.001 Catastrophising Working 8.59 N= 266 (only subjects with less than 65 years were selected for analysis) There are significant differences on mean scores between all groups on all the scales and questionnaires except for catastrophising. However, further examination revealed that there are only significant differences (p<0.001 Post Hoc analysis using Scheffe) on mean scores only between two groups (people working and not working due pain). 143 That is, lower scores on self-efficacy, depression and acceptance, and higher scores on disability were associated with unemployment. The ability to detect some differences among groups suggests that these measures have also concurrent validity and are capable of identifying distinct outcomes or groups. The next analysis examined the correlation of the different measures; including the 5 factors found for the Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire. Due to the large number of correlations (66), it was necessary to adjust the Alpha level and to choose a parameter to avoid the increased chances of a Type I error. To avoid Type I error, a Bonferroni adjustment was applied to the level of significance (p=0.05/66). Therefore, only correlations at a p=0.001 were considered significant. Low to moderate correlations found among different measures suggest that these measures have adequate discriminant validity and measure different dimensions. Moderate correlations may indicate a relationship between some of these variables (e.g. depression and catastrophising). The higher correlations found between Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire and the Activity Engagement Scale suggests the total score is more influenced by this sub-scale than the Pain Willingness sub-scale. 144 Table 3.15 Correlations between questionnaires, scales and CPAQ factors Questionnaires RMDQ RMDQ PSEQ DASS CAT Scale Scale CPAQ AE PW Scale Scale F1 F2 F3 F4 F5 - PSEQ -0.58 - Depression Scale 0.34 -0.38 - Catastrophising Scale 0.34 -0.39 0.59 - CPAQ -0.39 0.64 -0.44 -0.47 - Activity Engagement Scale -0.33 0.67 -0.38 -0.38 0.79 - Pain Willingness Scale -0.23 0.22 -0.24 -0.29 0.64 0.05^ - Factor 1 -0.37 0.69 -0.40 -0.39 0.78 0.95 0.08^ - Factor 2 -0.25 0.22 -0.19 -0.25 0.61 0.65 0.91 0.09^ - Factor 3 -0.08^ 0.10 -0.22 -0.24 0.43 -0.34 0.74 0.01^ 0.44 Factor 4 -0.12 -0.18 -0.23 0.58 0.75 0.22^ 0.54 0.06^ Factor 5 -0.10^ 0.17 0.35 -0.16 0.26 -0.18 0.40 -0.16 -0.09 0.17 -0.09^ -0.08^ -0.09^ - All correlations were significant at the 0.001 level except for the ones marked with ^ which were above 0.05 145 There is a moderate correlation between factors 1 and 4, and between factors 2 and 3. But also there is no correlation or a very low and low correlation between factor 5 and all other factors (range from -0.09 to 0.26). In addition, there were high to very high correlations between factors 1 and 4 and the acceptance scale (AE) and between factors 2 and 3 and the pain willingness scale (PW), which suggests that these four factors form two distinct scales. When examining the correlations between all the factors and the CAPQ total score, only factor 1 (activity engagement) had a high correlation with CAPQ, while factors 2, 3 and 4 have only a moderate correlation, and factor 5 has a low correlation. These results suggest that factor 1 is the most relevant factor of the CPAQ questionnaire and also that correlation between factors and scales differ slightly from the results reported by McCracken (1999, 2004), which will be considered further in the discussion. 146 3.6 Discussion This study examined the psychometric properties of a number of translated measures widely used for assessing psychosocial factors considered relevant for evaluating chronic pain patients. The summary of the findings and the discussion are presented separately for each questionnaire. 3.6.1 Psychometric properties of the RMDQ Brazilian version The Roland and Morris Disability Questionnaire – Brazilian version for general chronic pain (abbreviated as RMDQ - BrGCP) had a mean score of 12.03 (SD=6.21), which does not seem too different from the mean score reported by Roland and Morris’ original validation for LBP ( X =11.4 but SD was not reported in this study and therefore t test could not be calculated) (Roland and Morris, 1983). The Brazilian sample’s mean score was also below the cut-off point expected for high disability in back pain patients reported by these authors in their original validation. When compared to a Brazilian validation for back pain patients ( X =14.30, SD=7.47) (Nusbaun et al, 2001) the mean score in the present study ( X =12.03, SD=6.21) was lower than the one reported by these authors, but not significantly different (t=1.62, p<0.05). 147 Reliability was tested using internal consistency coefficients and correlation between forms (split-half). The RMDQ (BrGCP) had a Cronbach’s α of 0.90 and a Pearson correlation coefficient between forms of 0.82, which are similar to the internal consistency values reported by the literature 0.92 (Asghari and Nicholas 2001; Jensen et al. 1992), 0.90 (Patrick et al. 1995), and 0.89 (Grotle et al. 2003). Furthermore, according to Jensen (2003) correlation coefficients in this range can be considered adequate for clinical and research purposes. Criterion validity was examined through comparisons of means between different groups regarding working status and level of education. The mean score on the RMDQ (BrGCP) on the working group, partially working and not working group was significantly different (p < 0.001). This finding is similar to those reported in the literature, that support an association between levels of disability on self-report and work status (Robinson 2001; Turner et al. 2003). There were also significant differences on means between the group with high levels of education and the 4 to 8 years of education group (means were 12.03 and 14.55 respectively, f=17.4 at a P=0.001). The discrepancy on education may be reflected by the type of work these two groups do, with the more educated generally doing less physical work. These differences on mean score between groups suggest that this measure is sensitive to detect differences between distinct groups or subjects under different conditions. These results support the RMDQ Brazilian version criterion validity which indicates the efficacy of this 148 measure to predict behaviour (i.e. work status) (Anastasi and Urbina, 1997). These findings are also similar to several studies (Nusbaum et al. 2001; Patrick et al. 1995; Roland and Fairbank 2000), which provided further support for the validity and sensitivity of the RMDQ. Correlation coefficients with other tests that measure different constructs were examined to test the discriminant properties of this measure. It is expected that a test should not have a high correlation with measures of different constructs (Anastasi and Urbina, 1997). The RMDQ (BrGCP) correlation coefficient (Pearson) with PSEQ, DASSdepression scale, PRSS-catastrophising scale, CPAQ and its scale and factors were in most cases significant (p<0.001), but this level of significance is partially inflated by the large number of subjects (i.e. it is easy to achieve a significant correlation when testing the relationships among a high number of variables with a moderate to high number of subkects n=311). However, the actual correlations varied from very low to moderate. These statistics suggest that there is a relation between some of these constructs, but at the same time there is not a complete overlap between these measures. The highest RMDQ (BrGCP) correlation was with the PSEQ (0.58), which is supported by other findings (Nicholas and Asghari, 2006) that reported an influence of self-efficacy on disability measure by the PSEQ and the RMDQ. These findings are also similar to others found in the literature (Asghari and Nicholas 2001; Kovacs et al. 2004; Stroud et al. 2004). The correlation between the RMDQ (BrGCP) and the average pain intensity (reported in a VNS varying form 0 to 10) was significant but 149 also low (r=0.38), which suggests that there is a relation between pain intensity and disability but that they are distinct constructs and also that disability is not only a function of pain intensity. This finding is also supported by several studies (Linton 2000; Pincus et al. 2002; SwinklesMeewisse et al. 2006; Williams 2001). The findings related to the psychometric properties of the RMDQ (BrGCP) supported by other studies, suggest that this measure is valid and reliable in the Brazilian sample with chronic pain and may be used in other Brazilian chronic pain populations. 3.6.2 Psychometric properties of the PRSS–Catastrophising Scale Brazilian version Regarding its descriptive statistics, the PRSS – Catastrophising Scale Brazilian version (Cat-Br) had a mean score 2.38 (SD=1.38), which differs significantly from the results (2.03 mean, SD= 1.22, t=3.18, p< 0.05) reported by Flor et al., (1993). Reliability of the PRSS – Cat-Br was tested using internal consistency coefficient and correlation between forms (split-half). The Catastrophising scale had a Cronbach’s α of 0.89, which is very similar to the results reported by Flor et al. (1993) of 0.92. These authors also reported a test-re-test correlation of 0.87, while results from this study found a Pearson correlation between split-half forms of 0.74. These correlation coefficients can be considered adequate within the 150 recommended range of 0.70 to 0.90 (Jensen 2003), and suggest that this measure is consistent and reliable to be used in this population. The construct validity of the Catastrophising scale was examined using principal component analysis (PCA) and discriminant validity analysis. PCA indicated the existence of two factors named rumination and helplessness. These factors accounted for 54.5% and 12.9% respectively, with factors loading varying from moderate to high (0.68 to 0.98), except for item 3 which loaded in the rumination factor, but with a low communality and loading (0.25 and 0.38). These findings are similar to the ones reported by Sullivan et al. (1995, 2006) who found three factors on the PCS – Pain Catastrophising Scale, named rumination, magnification and helplessness, accounting for a 41%, 10% and 8 % variance respectively. D’Leon et al. (2004) also reported the existence of three factors. Other studies have found a two-factor model of the PCS (rumination and powerlessness, which is a combination of magnification and helplessness) (Chibnall and Tait 2005). Internal consistency (Cronbach alpha values) for two factors were similar to the one found for the whole scale (0.86 and 0.85). Mean interitem correlations (0.55 and 0.58) also support the existence of two factors, suggesting as previously described by Sullivan and D’eon (1990), that these factors are different dimensions of this construct. The Catastrophising scale correlation coefficient (Pearson) with PSEQ, DASS - Depression scale, RMDQ, CPAQ and its scales were in most cases significant (p<0.001) but varied from low to moderate (0.29 to 0.59), with the highest correlation occurring with the depression scale. 151 These results indicate that there is not a large overlap between these measures and that catastrophising is conceptually distinct from depression. These findings are supported by several previous findings by other researchers (Sullivan et al., 1995; D’Leon et al., 2004; Geisser et al., 1994; Turner and Leslie, 2001; Jones et al., 2003). Altogether, the 2 factors found in this study, the internal consistency coefficient and the correlation coefficient between the two factors, are in consonance with recent studies (D'eon et al. 2004; Sullivan et al. 2005). Criterion validity was examined through comparisons of means between different groups. There were no significant differences on means between groups regarding level of education and working status. This seems to be appropriate, since catastrophic thoughts should not be related with level of education and might not be associated with work status as well. Altogether these results support the validity and reliability of this measure, which will be also, explored in Study 2 and 3. 3.6.3 Psychometric properties of the DASS – Depression Scale Brazilian version The DASS – Depression Scale Brazilian version (DASS-Dep-Br) had a mean score of 14.03 (SD: 12.02), while the original version for general population had a mean of 7.19 (SD=6.54) (Lovibond and Lovibond 1995), and the mean for chronic pain patients was 15.15 (SD=12.22) 152 (Taylor et al. 2005). As expected, there was a significant difference between the mean score for the general sample and the Brazilian chronic pain sample, but no significant difference between the Brazilian and the Australian chronic pain samples (t=1.23, expected t is 1.96, p =0.05). Reliability of the DASS-Depression Brazilian version was also tested using internal consistency coefficients and correlation between forms (split-half). The depression scale had a Cronbach coefficient of 0.96, which is very high, but very similar to the results reported by Taylor et al. (2005) in other samples (Alpha coefficients for general population was 0.95, for clinical 0.96 and on chronic pain subjects was 0.96). A recent study proposing a short-version for the DASS with half of the original items reported an internal consistency coefficient of 0.88 (Henry and Crawford 2005). In this study using the same items to run a split-half correlation between forms, a Pearson correlation coefficient of 0.93 was found. This is also a high correlation and similar to the above finding and support the reliability of the DASS-Depression-Br. But, unfortunately there are no normative data published for the DASS, which would enable further comparisons. The construct validity of the DASS-Depression-Br was examined using principal component analysis, discriminant and criterion validity. PCA indicated the existence of only one factor that assesses only cognitive symptoms of depression. This one factor solution accounted for 64.4% of total variance. High loadings (range .73 to .87) for all items except for one (.64) and high communality among items confirms the existence of only one factor on the DASS – depression scale. This result 153 is similar to Taylor et al. (2005) and Lovibond and Lovibond (1995) which confirms the unidimensionality of this scale. As described earlier in the literature review, a number of studies have reported the existence of some distinct dimensions while analyzing self-reports of depression (i.e. BDI, Zung) (Novy et al. 1995; Pincus and Williams 1999; Robinson and Riley III 1999). This aspect has been considered important due to the presence of somatic symptoms of depression and the overlap of symptoms between chronic pain and depression. Thus, this would generate an inflated score when using measures that have somatic symptoms of depression to evaluate patients with chronic pain, causing a biased result or diagnosis (Lovibond and Lovibond 1995; Pincus et al. 2004; Taylor et al. 2005). Based on this assumption, again the results presented earlier showing the existence of only one factor on the DASS-Depression-Br that assess solely cognitive symptoms of depression, enhances the importance of using this measure to assess depression in chronic pain patients and the goodness of fit of the DASS-Depression-Br for this purpose. Regarding the discriminant validity of the DASS-Depression-Br, its Pearson correlation coefficient with PSEQ, PRSS-catastrophising scale, RMDQ, CPAQ and its scales were in most cases significant (p<0.001) but varied from low to moderate (0.29 to 0.59), with the highest correlation occurring with the catastrophising scale. Again, this finding suggests that these scales assess different constructs. 154 Criterion validity was examined through comparisons of means between different groups. No significant differences were found between groups with different levels of education on the Depression Scale, but there are differences in means between different groups depending on their work status. Subjects not working due to pain had higher scores than the partially working and the working group (F=11.91, p=0.01). These results support the concurrent properties of the DASS-DepressionBr, and are somehow similar to the findings reported by Taylor et al. (2005) when they compared the scores of a sample composed by chronic pain patients, patients attending a psychology clinic and general population, who had significant differences. The significant differences found between different working groups and the lack of difference between groups with distinct levels of education is appropriate, since depression should not be expected to correlate with level of education, but might be expected to be associated with work status. This finding have been reported in the literature, that often report a correlation between depression and disability which appears to be mediated by other factors (Banks and Kerns 1996; Keefe et al. 2001; Pincus and Williams 1999). The relationship between psychological factors and disability will be further explored in Study 2. Altogether, these findings supported by other studies suggest that the DASS-Dep-Br is a valid and reliable measure to be used in the Brazilian sample with chronic pain and may be used for other Brazilian chronic pain populations. 155 3.6.4 Psychometric properties of the Pain Self-Efficacy Questionnaire - PSEQ Brazilian version The Pain Self-Efficacy Questionnaire - PSEQ Brazilian version (PSEQ - Br) had a mean of 34.84 (SD=14.08), while in a recent study with two different chronic pain samples the mean was 25.8 (SD=12.4) and (23.5 SD=13.1) (Nicholas 2007) . Although the results from the two samples presented in Nicholas’ study did not differ, the mean from the PSEQ-Br differ significantly (p=0.01) from both Australian samples. As noted before, differences between an Australian and a Brazilian sample will be examined in Study 3. Reliability of the PSEQ-Br was tested using internal consistency coefficients and correlation between forms (split-half). The PSEQ-Br had a Cronbach Alpha coefficient of 0.90, which is very similar to r=0.92 reported by Nicholas (1989). In the present study a split-half correlation between forms were also run, with a Pearson correlation coefficient of 0.76. The PSEQ-Br’s Cronbach alpha for internal consistency and the split-half correlation coefficient can be considered adequate and therefore supports its reliability (Tabachnick and Fidell 2001). The construct validity of the PSEQ-Br was examined using PCA and discriminant validity analyses. A principal component analysis indicated the existence of only one factor accounting for 55.29% of total variance 156 and an eigenvalue of 5.53. Median to high loadings (range .67 to .83) was found for all items except for item seven (.44, “I can cope with my pain without medication”). Item seven also shared a low communality with the other items while the other items shared a median to moderate communality. These results are similar to Nicholas (2007), who reported a total variance of 58.6%, median to high item-total correlations and a moderate factor loading, and the smallest coefficients’ on item seven. However as reported by Nicholas (2007) and (Ralphs et al. 1994) item seven has a high correlation with other measures. On the other hand, this result is different to Anderson et al. (1995) findings who reported a three factor solution for their Chronic Pain SelfEfficacy Scale (CPSS has 22 items) named: self-efficacy for pain management, self-efficacy for physical functioning and self-efficacy for coping with symptoms. However, there is some divergence related to the domains and content of self-efficacy these two scales assess. The main difference of the PSEQ and the CPSS is that the PSEQ asks how confident the respondent is that he/she can do things despite their pain. While in the CPSS all the questions ask how certain people are in managing their pain, do physical activities or cope with symptoms without emphasising that they must take into account how confident they are of doing things when in pain, which seems to be a important distinction. As expected, these two scales might be assessing different constructs or domains of self-efficacy beliefs. The PSEQ seems to be more specific than the CPSS and focus more on the beliefs of having 157 abilities to function while in pain, and not to manage pain or cope with its symptoms as the CPSS does. However, comparing these two scales was not the focus of this study. Regarding its discriminant validity, the PSEQ-Br correlation coefficient (Pearson) with the Depression scale, the catastrophising scale, the RMDQ, the CPAQ and its scale was in most cases significant (p<0.001), but varied from low to moderate (0.29 to 0.59), with negative correlations with measures of depression, disability and catastrophising. The relationship between depression and self-efficacy has also been reported by Anderson et al. (1995) who found a moderate correlation between their self-efficacy scale and depression (-0.42 to 0.62), and Arnstein et al. (1999) who found that self-efficacy contributes to depression. Furthermore, PSEQ-Br had its highest correlation (0.67 and 0.64 respectively) with CPAQ and its’ activity engagement scale, which theoretically measure acceptance and with the RMDQ that measures disability. These low to moderate correlations suggest that the PSEQ-Br measures different constructs than these instruments, supporting its construct validity (Campbell and Fiske 1959). This findings also indicate that self-efficacy beliefs might contribute to disability, which is supported by the literature (Arnstein et al. 1999; Asghari and Nicholas 2001; DeGood 2000; Nicholas 2007; Williams et al. 1994). The relationship between self-efficacy and acceptance is discussed in the next study. Criterion validity of the PSEQ-Br was examined through comparisons of means between different groups. Significant differences 158 were found between distinct educational groups (p<0.05), as well as with subjects under different work status (p<0.01). Participants with higher level of education (i.e. Tertiary) had higher means on the PSEQ-BR than subjects with four to eight years of education. Subjects in the working group had higher means than those in the not working due to pain group. These differences will be explored further in Study 2 and 3, but it could be hypothesised that level of education is related to work status. Beside this, level of education may contribute to self-efficacy. Low self-efficacy may also contribute to unemployment of chronic pain patients. No matter what the relationships are between these variables detecting differences among groups supports PSEQ-Br’ criterion validity. Altogether these findings indicate that the PSEQ-BR has sound psychometric properties in this Brazilian sample with chronic pain and may be used for other Brazilian chronic pain populations. 3.6.5 Psychometric properties of the Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire - CPAQ Brazilian version The Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire - CPAQ Brazilian version (CPAQ-Br) had a mean score of 60.20 (SD=18.11), the activity engagement scale mean was 39.89 (SD=14.07) and the pain willingness scale mean score was 20.21 (SD=11.08). McCracken and colleagues’ version (1999) had a mean of 56.3 (SD=20.4), the activity engagement scale mean was 33.3 (SD=13.2) and the pain willingness scale mean was 13.8 (SD=7.9). A study from Nicholas and Asghari (2006) reported a 159 mean of 53.8 (SD=18.0) for the CPAQ total score. The CPAQ Brazilian version mean score was higher and differ significantly from both studies (t=2.30 and t=2.75 at p<0.05 respectively) and may raise a number of hypotheses discussed at the end of this chapter and examined in Studies 2 and 3. Reliability of the CPAQ-Br including its scale was tested using internal consistency coefficients and correlation between split-half forms. The Cronbach α for the CPAQ-Br (total score), the activity engagement, pain willingness, thought control and chronicity factors were respectively 0.78, 0.87, 0.75, 0.50 and 0.58 which are similar to the results reported by McCracken (1999) 0.89, 0.90, 0.79, 0.74 and 0.58 with a 22 item version and McCracken et al. (2004) 0.78, 0.82, 0.78, 0.64 and 0.62, with a 28 item version. The results from this study added to McCracken’s findings suggest an adequate degree of internal consistency (Cronbach Alpha > 0.70) for the whole scale, factors 1 and 2, but not for factors 3 and 4. Indeed the results from this study are similar to McCracken (2004), but differ from McCracken (1999) on relation to factor 3. The correlation coefficient for the CPAQ and its two major scales, was 0.68 (CPAQ), 0.73 (AE) and 0.57 (PW), which can be considered a little below the adequate correlation coefficients recommended range of 0.70 to 0.90 (Jensen 2003), especially for the pain willingness scale. Altogether these findings suggest that the reduction of items proposed by McCracken (1999 and 2004) was reasonable, but that further analyses regarding the reliability and validity of the CPAQ and its scales should be conducted. 160 The construct validity of the CPAQ-Br was examined using principal component analysis (PCA), item-scale correlations and discriminant validity analysis. PCA using an eigenvalue criterion greater than 1 initially indicated the presence of five factors named: activity engagement, pain willingness, thought control, chronicity and accepting pain. This fivefactor solution accounted for 57.2% of the variance. But based on previous results of a four factor solution (McCracken, 1999 and McCracken et al, 2004), face validity and clinical experience (item 3 the only item in factor five was the most missed item on data collection), a final four-factor solution was thought to be more appropriated. This fourfactor solution accounted for 51% of variance, which is similar to the 52.7% of variance found by McCracken (1999) and 46.8% variance in (2004) with two factors. Based on these further analyses were conducted using a four-factor solution and also examining the psychometric properties of the CPAQ as proposed by McCracken et al. (2004). Low mean inter-item correlation also confirmed a poor correlation among the items in all factors, except for factor 1, which has a moderate correlation (0.48). Regarding item-scale correlations, most correlations between items and the two relevant scales were moderate (Pearson correlation range 0.44 to 0.78 at a p< 00.1), except for item 3 which had a correlation of 0.35 with factor one and 0.14 with factor two (items were not adjusted). This result supports the reliability of factor one but not for the other factors. Discriminant validity was examined through the CPAQ-Br and its two scales, and the five factors solution presented earlier and the 161 correlation coefficient (Pearson) with PSEQ, DASS - Depression scale, RMDQ and PRSS – Catastrophising scale. In most of cases significant correlations were found between several tests, scales and factors (p<0.01) in the expected directions, as well as a very low correlation coefficient between some measures. The correlations between the five factors for the CPAQ-Br, the activity engagement scale and the pain willingness scale are presented below: a. Factor five had a very low to low correlations with all other factors, as well as with the CPAQ-Br and its’ two scales. This result confirms the preliminary findings of this study (low inter-item and item-scale correlations, and split-half coefficient and PCA) that supported only a four factor solution; b. Factor four had a moderate correlation with factor one (0.53), the CPAQ-Br total score (0.58) and a high correlation with the activity engagement scale (0.75). c. Factor three had a moderate correlation with factor two (0.44) and with the CPAQ-Br total score, and a high correlation (0.74) with the pain willingness scale. d. Factor two correlated moderately with factor 3 (0.44) and the CPAQBr total score (0.61), and very highly with the pain willingness scale (0.91). e. Factor one had a moderate correlation with factor four (0.53), a high correlation with CPAQ-Br total score and a very high correlation with the activity engagement scale (0.95). 162 These results occurred in the expected directions, since factor one and two had the majority of items from the activity engagement scale and the pain willingness scale (7 out of 11 and 6 out of 9 respectively). The high correlation between factor one and four, factor two and three and its respective scales supports McCracken and colleague’s (2004) proposal for the existence of two scales in the CPAQ, composed by two factors each, but as noted previously with a different number of items on each factor in different studies. High to moderate correlations (0.78 to 0.41) between factor one, two, three and four with CPAQ-Br total score suggest that there is more than two dimensions on the CPAQ-Br. But that the main dimension probably is captured by factor 1, which has seven items from the original activity engagement scale. The high correlation between the CPAQ-Br and the activity engagement scale (0.80) and moderate correlation between the CPAQ-Br and the pain willingness scale (0.64) also enhance the importance of the first scale when compared to the other scale. These findings are similar to those reported by McCracken (1999), who reported a correlation coefficient of (0.92) for factor 1 and (0.74) for factor 4 with the CPAQ total score. McCracken et al. (2004) also reported a correlation of 0.80 for the activity engagement scale and (0.76) for the pain willingness scale with the CPAQ total score. Nicholas and Asghari (2006) have also found similar correlations between the activity engagement factor (0.85) and the pain willingness factor (0.76) with the CPAQ total score. These high correlations between the two more relevant 163 factors and the total score partially support the construct validity of this measure, the existence of distinct dimensions in the CPAQ-Br, and the possibility of reducing the number of items for the CPAQ-Br version. Correlations between the CPAQ-Br and its scales with other measures were also examined to evaluate discriminant validity. Correlations between the CPAQ-Br, the activity engagement and pain willingness scale and the RMDQ, the Depression scale, the catastrophising scale and the PSEQ varied from low to moderate (at a p<0.01 range from 0.23 to 0.67), suggesting that the CPAQ-Br and its scales measure different construct. The highest correlations were obtained between the self-efficacy measure and the activity engagement scale (0.67). On the other hand, in contrast with the activity engagement scale and the total CPAQ, the pain willingness scale had a low correlation with self-efficacy (0.22), but the same low correlations with depression, catastrophising and physical disability. These results support McCracken (1999), Nicholas and Asghari (2006) findings that reported a low correlation between physical disability, depression and CPAQ and its scales. These last authors also found a similar relationship between the CPAQ total score and its scales and self-efficacy. Viane et al. (2003) also did not found a correlation between acceptance and catastrophising. These correlations suggest that that the activity engagement scale seems to be the more psychometrically sound scale of the CPAQ and also that there is a relevant relationship between some cognitions and acceptance, which has not yet being fully understood. 164 Criterion validity was examined through comparisons of means between different groups. No significant differences (p≤0.01) were found between the CPAQ and its scales on relation to education level, but there were significant differences between groups under different working conditions (working and not working due to pain) on the two scales and the CPAQ-Br total score. These findings suggest that the CPAQ-Br and its scales are sensitive to detect differences among groups. As noted before, a few differences on mean score between groups were found on the tests. The relationships between these socio-economic and psychosocial variables examined in this study are investigated in Study 2 and 3. 165 3.6.6 Summary of discussion The results of the reliability (internal consistency and split-half correlations), construct validity, convergent validity, criterion and discriminant validity analyses suggest that the Brazilian version of the examined measures: the Roland and Morris Disability Questionnaire, the Pain Self-Efficacy Questionnaire, the DASS – Depression Scale, the PRSS – Catastrophising Scale, the Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire and the activity engagement and pain willingness scale have in general sound psychometric properties in this sample, and may indeed be used in the Brazilian chronic pain populations. The results of the present study support previous findings (Flor et al. 1993; Lovibond and Lovibond 1995; McCracken et al. 1999; McCracken et al. 2004b; Nicholas 1989; Nicholas 2007; Roland and Fairbank 2000; Roland and Morris 1983; Taylor et al. 2005) suggesting that all the studied measures are valid, reliable and that psychometric norms can be established to be used in the Brazilian chronic pain population. However in relation to the CPAQ Brazilian version, it may be prudent to use only the activity engagement scale (which consist of factors 1 and 4) with a total of 10 items and eliminating item 3. This hypothesis will be tested on Study 2 and 3 when the predictive properties of acceptance (measured by the CPAQ-Br) will be examined and further conclusions would be drawn. 166 All the other measures seem to be used in a similar way proposed initially by researchers. Furthermore, other studies conducted in Brazil with these standardised measures will also elucidate their properties and collaborate to test and enhance their validity and reliability. The limitations of the present study should also be acknowledged. These may include data collection procedures related to psychometric aspects and sample characteristics. In relation to the first aspect, testing the reliability of the questionnaires could not be done through test-re-test due to the difficulties in contacting subjects for a second application of the questionnaires; however other reliability analyses were conducted. Another limitation of this research concerns the sample employed. First the sample was mainly constituted of chronic pain patients attending pain clinics. This may produce some biased information, since this sample may consist of subjects that did not respond to pain as successfully as a nonclinical pain population. Second, our sample was not intended to be representative of the Brazilian population with chronic pain. It is also important to acknowledge that some of the differences found between the present study and other studies mentioned may be due to differences in samples, cultural differences and health system contingencies. As noted before, a number of relationships between the studied factors as well as cultural differences are explored in Study 2 and Study 3. Those results may also contribute to examine the psychometric properties of the used measures. 167 CHAPTER FOUR Study Two: The relative contributions of self-efficacy beliefs, catastrophising and acceptance to disability and depression in a Brazilian population with chronic pain. 4.1 Introduction Research suggests that cognitive, emotional and social factors may shape the individual’s pain experience, influencing the degree to which pain is experienced, responses to it, and the degree of interference caused by it (Eccleston 2001; Skevington 1995; Turk 1996). Disability in people with chronic pain appears to be related at least or as much to psychological and social factors as to pathophysiology (Pincus et al. 2002; Rudy et al. 2003; Turk 2002b). Some attitudes, cognitive styles, fear-avoidance beliefs, passive coping approaches, pain cognitions (i.e. catastrophising) may actually be considered risk factors to future physical disability and emotional maladjustment (Linton, 2000). Depression in people with chronic pain also appears to be strongly influenced by cognitive variables, such as catastrophising (Nicholas and Asghari 2006; Sullivan et al. 2001a), self-efficacy beliefs (Nicholas and Asghari 2006; Turk and Okifuji 2002), acceptance (McCracken and Eccleston 2003) and disability (Pincus and Williams 1999; Worz 2003). There is also evidence that there is a bi-directional relationship between these variables (Pincus and Williams 1999; Turk et al. 1995). 168 As noted previously, the vast majority of the research on the relationship between chronic pain and adjustment and the role of psychological variables has come from high income countries in Western countries. However a few studies have been conducted in the Brazilian chronic pain population (da Silva et al. 2004; Pimenta 2001). One study conducted with a small sample (n=79) has done a preliminary cross-cultural validation of the Coping Strategies Questionnaire - CSQ (Keefe and Williams 1990), which includes the catastrophising scale. The role of catastrophising on disability and depression was not the scope of this study. The findings suggested that this measure is valid and reliable for this population and that there were no major cultural differences on interpreting the items and responses occurred in a similar trend (Portnoi et al. 2005). Another study (n=132) has been conducted on pain-related selfefficacy beliefs (Salvetti and Pimenta 2004) using the Chronic Pain selfefficacy Scale - CPSS (Anderson et al. 1995). The results yielded a 3 factor solution and reported adequate psychometric properties, confirming the initial validation of this measure. The authors also found a moderate and negative correlation with depression measured by the BDI. Furthermore, the Brazilian sample also had a mean score significantly higher than the sample used in the initial validation of the CPSS. The concept of acceptance and its relationship with chronic pain has not been investigated yet. On the other hand, the relationship between depression and chronic pain has been the most common studied factor. 169 Although, there is a lack of epidemiological studies in this population in Brazil, some studies have reported a prevalence of depression around 50% (Figueiró 1999; Lorencatto et al. 2002; Novello et al. 2005); which does not differ from other chronic pain population. To date no research in Brazil has reported investigating depression in a chronic pain population using a depression scale without somatic items. Based on this evidence, it is clear that further research in this area will be helpful to test the generalizability of the psychological research findings from other countries and cultures and confirm if they can be applied to a Brazilian chronic pain population. Accordingly, the intention in conducting this study is to explore the relationship among self-efficacy, catastrophising, acceptance, depression and disability in a Brazilian population with chronic pain. If the findings are consistent with those reported in the pain literature generally, it would provide further support for a biopsychosocial perspective on chronic pain. This, in turn, would suggest that clinical treatment procedures developed in other countries might be used effectively in the Brazilian population. 170 4.2 Method 4.2.1 Subjects Data from 311 participants described in Study 1 were analysed to test the relationship among the variables previously described. 4.2.2 Inclusion and exclusion criteria Participants were included or excluded in this study based on the criteria described in Study One. 4.2.3 Procedure Data collected from all the subjects were entered into a database for statistical analyses using SPSS 14 for Windows. 4.2.4 Measures As described in Chapter Three, data were collected using the following questionnaires: the Roland and Morris Disability Questionnaire (Roland and Morris 1983), the Pain Self-Efficacy Questionnaire (Nicholas 1989), the PRSS - Catastrophising Scale (Flor et al. 1993), the Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire (McCracken et al. 2004b), the DASS Depression scale (Lovibond and Lovibond 1993), a socio demographic, and a clinical inventory. All the psychometric properties of these measures have already been described in Study One, and all have adequate validity and reliability. 171 4.3 Hypothesis This study is intended to test three hypotheses. (1) Pain self-efficacy beliefs, acceptance, catastrophising and depression are controlling for significant predictors socio-demographic of and disability clinical after variables, including average pain intensity. (2) Pain self-efficacy beliefs, acceptance, catastrophising and disability controlling are for significant predictors socio-demographic of and depression clinical after variables, including average pain intensity. (3) Socio-demographic and psychological variables are risk factors for unemployment due to chronic pain. 4.4 Aims of study 1. To analyse the predictive properties of the psychological and demographic variables on disability, depression, pain intensity and work status in a Brazilian population with chronic pain; 2. To examine whether the relationships among pain related selfefficacy beliefs, acceptance, catastrophizing on depression and disability in a Brazilian chronic pain population are similar to the findings reported in the literature and consistent with current biopsychosocial models of chronic pain. 172 4.5 Statistical analyses A number of statistical analyses, mainly t-tests4, analyses of variance (ANOVA), multiple correlation, multiple hierarchical regression and logistic regression analyses will be used to test the relationship between the variables described above and its predictive properties (Tabachnick and Fidell, 2001). All the analyses were conducted using the SPSS for Windows version 14.0. 4 Whenever a t-test was conducted the assumption of equal variance between independent variables was examined by Levene’s test for equality of variance. 173 4.6 Results Initially data distribution was tested for skewed distributions and kusrtosis (with all variables not exciding the expected range –1 to 1), multicollinearity and singularity using SPSS-14. All data were normally distributed, variables were not highly correlated and not redundant, thus statistical transformations were not necessary. However, due to the coding system used on data entry some variables were transformed to dummy variables. Work status initially was categorised into not working due to pain, partially working and working, but for correlation and regression analyses the first category was coded as 1, while both groups that were working (i.e. partially working and working) were coded as 2. Similarly the variable pain site was re-coded; all pain single sites were coded as 1, while pain in two or more sites was coded 2. A preliminary analysis using t-tests was conducted to test for differences between genders and number of pain sites on measures of disability, self-efficacy, depression, catastrophising and acceptance. There were no significant differences between genders on the mean scores on disability, self-efficacy, depression, catastrophising and both factors of acceptance (t≤1.96, p=0.05). However there was a significant difference (t=2.7 p=0.007) between genders on pain intensity. Males had a lower mean score ( X =5.5, SD=2.33), than females ( X =6.4, SD=2.37). 174 There were also no significant differences regarding number of pain sites on most measures. However, patients with pain in two or more sites had higher scores on disability and depression (p<0.001). Disability mean score for patients with pain in two or more sites was 13.42 (SD=5.81), while for pain in any other site was 10.90 (SD=6.31) (t = 4.8 p=0.001). Depression mean score for patients with pain in two or more sites was 17.16 (SD=12.65), and for other sites the mean was 11.47 (SD=10.87) (t = -2.8 p=0.005). The contributions of other variables such as work status and level of education on the psychological variables described above have been reported in Study One. As noted there were significant mean differences on the Roland and Morris Disability Questionnaire only; the group with higher education had significant lower means when compared with the 9 to 11 years of education group (p=0.001) and with the 4 to 8 years of education (p=0.001). Regarding work status, significant differences on mean scores were found between groups on all the measures, except for the Catastrophising scale. However, further analyses revealed that the only significant differences (p<0.01 Post Hoc analysis using Scheffe) on mean scores were between people working and not working due to pain. There were no significant differences for age and gender on level of education and work status. Correlational analyses were also conducted to examine the relationship among age, pain intensity, pain duration, disability, selfefficacy, depression, catastrophising and acceptance scales. 175 Due to the large number of correlations (45) it was necessary to adjust the Alpha level to avoid the increased chances of a Type I and Type II error5. To avoid Type I error, a Bonferroni adjustment was applied to the level of significance. In other words, the Alpha level was divided by the number of variables used in the analysis (i.e. 0.05/45 =0.001). Therefore, only correlations at this level were considered significant. 5 That is including false positives (Type I error) and excluding false negatives (Type II error) in the analysis. Therefore a balance between lowering the p level (0.01) or increasing p levels (0.0001). 176 Table 4.1 Pearson correlations among socio-demographic, clinical and psychological measures Age Age Pain Pain duration intensity Disability Depression Self-efficacy Catastrophising Activity Pain engagement willingness - Pain duration - 0.01 - Pain intensity - 0.08 - 0.01 - Disability - 0.06 0.08 0.30* - Depression - 0.13 0.18* 0.25* 0.34* - Self-efficacy 0.06 - 0.07 - 0.25* - 0.58* - 0.38* - - 0.16* 0.22* 0.28* 0.34* 0.59* - 0.39* - 0.16* - 0.09 - 0.11 - 0.33* - 0.38* 0.67* - 0.38* - 0.06 -0.10 - 0.07 - 0.23* - 0.24* 0.22* - 0.28* 0.05 - 0.17* -0.13 -0.12 -0.40* -0.44* 0.64* -0.47* 0.79* 0.64* Catastrophising Activity engagement Pain willingness Acceptance * Significant correlations at P ≤ 0.001 177 There were significant correlations among several variables. However, most of the significant correlations were low in size (less than 0.40) (Rowntree 1981). Only the correlations between disability and selfefficacy (-0.58), depression and catastrophising (0.59), and self-efficacy and activity engagement (0.67), and self-efficacy and the acceptance questionnaire (0.64) were in the moderate range (higher than 0.40). Correlations between the CPAQ and its scales were in the moderate to high range, as expected, due to the total CPAQ score being the sum of the two scales. Other correlations were nearly in the moderate range, such as, depression and self-efficacy (-0.38), depression and activity engagement (-0.38), catastrophising and activity engagement (-0.38) and self-efficacy and catastrophising (-0.39). Low correlations, but at statistically significant levels were found between age and catastrophising and activity engagement, pain duration and catastrophising and depression, pain intensity catastrophising, engagement and disability, disability and pain and depression depression, willingness, and self-efficacy catastrophising, between depression and activity and pain willingness, and between pain willingness and depression, catastrophising and self-efficacy. To examine the relationships among three chosen criterion variables (physical disability, depression and pain intensity), sociodemographic and clinic variables, and the psychological variables, a series of Hierarchical Multiple Regression Analysis (HMRA) were conducted. 178 Among the socio-demographic, clinic and psychological variables, only those with significant levels of correlation with the outcomes as established previously using a Bonferroni correction (P ≤ 0.001) were entered in the regression analyses and those with significant differences according to t-tests and analysis of variance. In each regression model, a block of socio-demographic (e.g. gender and level of education) and clinic variables (e.g. pain severity) were entered first in order to control for the associations between these variables and the dependent variable, followed by the cognitive and mood block of variables (enter method) (see Tables 2, 3 and 4). In the first regression analysis, although depression is one of the studied outcomes, due to its importance in relation to disability, depression was entered in step two as an independent variable with the other cognitive variables. In the second regression analysis, due to the important effect of disability on depression this variable was entered in step two as an independent variable with the other cognitive variables. To test the contribution of these variables to pain intensity, the sociodemographic variable (gender) was entered in the first step, depression but not disability was entered in a second step with the other cognitive variables (enter method). In order to control for the risk of Type I errors, a Bonferroni adjustment was applied for each group of analyses. Level of significance was adjusted dividing p at 0.05 by the number of variables entered in the regression analyses for each predictor. The adjusted p value for the each 179 group of variables was: disability (0.05/13 = 0.004), for depression (0.05/13 = 0.004), and for pain intensity (0.05/7 = 0.007). Table 4.2 Multiple hierarchical regression analysis predicting disability. Step and predictors Criterion Total R² F df R² Change F change Betaª t variable: Disability 0.22 Step1: 21.40** 306 0.22 21.40** Level of education -.19 -4.2** Gender .01 0.2 Pain intensity .11 2.4 Pain site .19 4.3** Depression .04 0.77 Active engagement .08 1.32 Pain willingness -.07 -1.52 Catastrophising .07 1.31 Self-Efficacy -.51 -8.23** Step 2: 0.47 29.47** 301 0.25 28.30** ª Standardised regression coefficient * P ≤ 0.004 ** p ≤ 0.001 In the first analysis (Table 4.2), physical disability measured by the RMDQ was significantly predicted by level of education, gender, pain intensity and pain site (i.e. presence of more than one pain site). These variables accounted for 22% of the variance in disability, but only educational level and pain site obtained significant p levels. In the second 180 step of the analysis, measures of depression, activity engagement, pain willingness, catastrophising and self-efficacy were entered into the regression equation as a block. These variables accounted for an additional 25% of variance in disability, however among these variables only self-efficacy beliefs were a significant predictor of disability (beta = .51, p<0.001). Table 4.3 Multiple hierarchical regression analysis predicting depression Step and predictors Criterion Total R² F df R² Change 0.11 9.86** 306 0.11 F change Betaª t variable: Depression Step1: 9.86** Gender -.10. -0.21 Pain duration .04 1.01 Pain intensity .76 0.12 Pain site .08 1.71 Disability .07 1.08 Active engagement -.15 -2.48 Pain willingness -.06 -1.38 Catastrophising .45 8.48** Self-Efficacy -.02 -0.24 Step 2: 0.41 23.40** 301 0.30 30.44** ª Standardised regression coefficient * P ≤ 0.004 ** p ≤ 0.001 181 In the second analysis (Table 4.3), depression measured by the DASS-Depression scale was predicted by the combination of gender, pain duration, pain intensity and pain site (accounting for 11% of the variance as a block). However, when disability and psychological variables were entered in the equation none of these variables obtained significant p levels. After controlling for these variables, disability and cognitive variables were entered in step two as a group. Altogether this group of variables explained an additional 30% of the variance. However, only catastrophising (beta = .45, p < 0.001) made a significant contribution to depression. In order to further investigate the relationship between catastrophising and depression, another MHRA was conducted by using the identified factors in Study One, helplessness and rumination as the independent variables, after controlling for gender and pain variables. The contribution of acceptance factors was not analysed due to its non-contribution to predict any outcome, and also due to the high correlations between the 4 factors, which would cause multicollinearity. In regression analyses described in Table 4.3.a the relevant sociodemographic and clinical variables were entered in step one and catastrophising factors were entered after in step two. In order to control for Type I error, a Bonferroni adjustment was also applied for this analysis. Level of significance was adjusted dividing p at 0.05 by the number of variables entered in the regression analyses. The adjusted p value for this analysis was (0.05/10 = 0.005). 182 Table 4.3.a MHRA predicting depression from catastrophising factors Step and predictors Betaª t Gender -.35 -0.15 Pain duration .02 0.49 Pain intensity .11 2.42 Pain site .10 2.32 .17 2.90* .44 7.73** Criterion Total R² F df 0.11 9.65** 306 R² Change F change variable: Depression Step1: Step 2: 0.40 33.41** 304 Catastrophising 0.11 0.29 9.65** 71.78** Factor 1 Catastrophising Factor 2 ª Standardised coefficient for the catastrophising regression * P ≤ 0.005 ** p ≤ 0.001 This MHRA of predictors of depression that excluded other cognitive variables and disability, revealed that catastrophising factors (r= 0.59) accounted for an additional 29% of the variance, over and above the contribution of gender and pain variables. Both catastrophising factors were significant predictors at the adjusted p level value, which confirms the results of the second set of analyses (Table 4.3). Factor 2, which was named helplessness, seems to be a stronger predictor of depression (beta = .44, p= 0.001) than factor 1, named rumination (beta = .17, p = 0.004). This finding is in consonance with the literature and will be discussed further in this section. 183 In the third main analysis (Table 4.4), socio-demographic and psychological variables were examined as predictors for pain intensity. As noted previously, although not a major outcome variable in this study, it was an option to examine the contribution of the studied variables to pain intensity. Table 4.4 Multiple hierarchical regression analysis predicting pain intensity Step and predictors Criterion Total R² F df R² Change 0.02 7.42* 309 0.02 F change Betaª t variable: Pain intensity Step1: 7.42* Gender .16 2.91* Active engagement .17 2.24 Pain willingness .05 0.87 Depression .12 1.73 Catastrophising .17 2.47 Self-Efficacy -.28 -3.82** Step 2: 0.15 9.04** 304 0.13 9.17** ª Standardised regression coefficient * P ≤ 0.007 ** p ≤ 0.001 The results of the third regression analysis (Table 4.4) show that gender explained only 2% of the variance in pain intensity (beta = .16, p = 0.007). Cognitive and mood variables entered in step 2 explained an additional 13% of the variance. However, only self-efficacy (beta = -.28, p < 0.001) made a significant contribution to the variance in pain 184 intensity. Although catastrophising contributed significantly to pain intensity at a p = 0.01 level, it was not significant when the Bonferroni correction (p < 0.007) was applied. To examine the contribution of socio-demographic, clinical and psychological variables to work status, a logistic regression was conducted. Correlations between the variables entered in the regression were examined. As reported in table 4.1, there were no correlations among variables superior to 0.67 (except between sub-scales of the same measure), which indicates that multicollinearity is not a problem here. This means it is valid to conduct a regression analysis. As most of the variables were re-coded into dummy variables correlations were examined again. All Pearson correlations were below 0.30, which confirmed the lack of multicollinearity even when the variables were re-coded. For the best interpretation of the analysis of logistic regression (i.e. the measures had different ranges) all the variables were re-coded as dichotomous. The dependent variable (work status) was coded as follows. Patients who were working or partially working were coded 0, and patients who were not working were coded 1. Since work status is likely to be affected by age, only participants aged more than 18 years and less than 65 years were selected for the analysis. Thus, the number of subjects was 222. Mean age of this group was used as the cut off point (i.e. 45) for the re-coded age variable (age less than 45 was coded 0 and ≥ 45 was coded 1). 185 Patients having pain in any site were given 0, while 1 was equal to pain in two or more pain sites. Pain duration was coded 0 for patients with pain for up to 5 years, and 1 for patients who were in pain for 6 years or more. Pain intensity cut off point was set at 5. Education was also coded as a dummy variable. Patients with less than 11 years of education or equivalent levels were coded as 1, while subjects with technical education or tertiary were coded as 0. All the disability and psychological measures were coded in the same direction using upper quartile score as a cut off point (i.e. scores lower than the third quartile was coded 0 and the upper quartile scores were coded 1). However, when higher scores on the measure was a positive outcome it was coded 0 and lower scores were coded 1 (i.e. selfefficacy and acceptance). Upper quartiles and lower quartiles in case were low scores indicated worse scores were choose instead of mean as a cut off point because they would indicated more severe dysfunction levels, which would not appear if mean was the cut off point. Reduced (i.e. parsimonious models) were produced by a process of backwards elimination of the independent variables. At each stage, the effect of dropping out an independent variable was assessed using the likelihood ratio test (G statistic) at a significant level p=0.05. Odds ratios, beta coefficients and confidence intervals were inspected at each stage to check for possible confounding or effect modification and the goodness of fit of the model (Hosmer and Lemeshow 1989). Step 6 (of seven) was the best fit model, and although not attaining significant p levels some variables were kept in to the 186 regression model due to their contribution to the best fit of the model. In this case if an independent variable with a p-value exceeding 0.05 improved the model fit it was retained in the reduced model. After observing these criteria and examining other possibilities for coding the variables, the best model was chosen. However preliminary analyses were run coding questionnaire scores on groups according percentile, quartiles and mean cut off point. Age was also coded in a number of different categories (i.e. 18 to 28, 29 to 39, 40 to 50 and 51 to 65 and as a continuous variable). The results found for the best-fit model did not differ significantly from others using different categorisations for age. Table 4.5 Logistic regression analysis for work status with adjusted odds ratio, p values and confidence intervals (Brazilian chronic pain sample) Variable Adjusted odds ratio* 95% C.I. P level Age (≥ 45 years = 1) 0.39 0.20 – 0.74 0.004 Educational level (≤ 11 years = 1) 3.49 1.81 – 6.74 0.001 Physical disability – RMDQ (≥ 13 = 1) 2.75 1.27 – 5.97 0.01 Self-efficacy - PSEQ (≤ 35 = 1) 2.52 1.06 – 6.00 0.04 Acceptance - CPAQ ª (≤ 60 = 1) 1.92 0.86 – 4.30 0.11 * Pain intensity, pain site, pain duration, gender, depression and catastrophising were also included in the regression. ª Retained for improved model fit. Among all the socio-demographic, clinical and psychological variables, only educational level, age, physical disability and self-efficacy 187 attained the unemployment significance due to level pain. (p The ≤ 0.05) most as potent risk risk factors for factor for unemployment due to pain was educational level (odds ratio=3.49 p=0.001). This means that those subjects with less than 11 years of education had nearly 3.5 times more chance of being unemployed than those with higher education. Physical disability measured by the Roland and Morris Disability Questionnaire (odds ratio=2.75 p=0.001) also had a high contribution; which means that patients who were in the top quartile score group had nearly three times more chance of being unemployed than those with lower scores. The same trend occurred with self-efficacy (odds ratio=2.5 p=0.04); patients with low self-efficacy scores for pain were 2.5 times more likely to be unemployed than those with high or median self-efficacy scores. People aged 45 or over were 61% less likely to be unemployed than those in the younger group (less than 45). Although, acceptance was kept in the model due to its contribution to the goodness of fit of the model it did not attain a significant p level (p=0.05). In this model, gender, pain intensity, pain duration, pain site, depression and catastrophising also did not seem to be risk factor for unemployment in patients with chronic pain. 188 4.6.1 Summary of findings In this sample of Brazilian chronic pain patients, disability was predicted by level of education, number of pain sites and self-efficacy. Depression was predicted by catastrophising only. When the contribution of Catastrophising factors were analysed separately, both factors were found to be significant predictors of depression, with helplessness being a stronger predictor of depression than rumination. Pain intensity was predicted by gender and self-efficacy. Among all socio-demographic, clinical and psychological variables only educational level, age, physical disability and self-efficacy were found to be significant risk factors for unemployment associated with chronic pain. 189 4.7 Discussion This section examines the findings of this study in the context of the existing literature regarding the contribution of socio-demographic variables, clinical variables and cognitive variables to physical disability, depression, pain intensity and work status. The limitations and strengths of this study and Study Three will be discussed in the final discussion. 4.7.1 Contribution of socio-demographic, clinical and psychological variables to disability The findings that a number of socio-demographic and psychological variables were significantly correlated with disability and were predictors of it is consistent with the published literature (Jensen et al. 2001b; Lame et al. 2005; McCracken and Eccleston 2005; Nicholas 2007; Stroud et al. 2000; Turner et al. 2000). These findings are given added significance when it is considered that such findings have not been reported previously amongst Brazilian chronic pain patients. The following section will examine the findings in each area. 190 4.7.1.1 Contribution of socio-demographic and clinical variables to disability The finding that pain intensity did not contribute to disability in the studied population when psychological variables are taken into account is consistent with other results reported in the literature. While some studies have reported that pain intensity is an important predictor of disability (Arnstein et al. 1999; Viane et al. 2003), at least 30% of the variance in disability is related to other factors (Jensen et al. 1994; Viane et al. 2003). The findings that numbers of pain site contributed to disability is also in accordance with the literature, although with some variance (Von Korff et al. 1992). Level of education was also a significant predictor of disability. This finding suggests that participants who have lower levels of education may be employed in jobs involving more physical demands and thus may be exposed to employment job factors involved on disability (i.e. lifting) (e.g. Waddell, 2003). It should be recognised that this series of studies was conducted in a developing country, where level of education can severely compromise work status. It could be hypothesised that level of education contributes to work status in at least 3 ways. 1). Participants with lower level of education are usually employed in jobs involving physical work, which in turn could contribute to disability more than jobs involving mental activity; 2). People with higher level of education could have more 191 economic resources to help then treating a chronic disease (i.e. use of private health system); and 3). Level of education could moderate coping skills. All these hypotheses have social and clinical implications and deserve further research, but they are not the focus of this series of studies. Altogether these findings support the conclusion that in Brazilian chronic pain patients, as in chronic pain patients in other countries, although disability might be influenced by clinical factors (e.g. pain intensity), there is a high correlation between disability and lower socioeconomic status (Aggarwal et al. 2003; Blyth et al. 2001; Gureje et al. 1998; Hagen et al. 2006; Pimenta 2001; Volinn 1997). 4.7.1.2 Contribution of psychological variables to disability Among all the psychological variables studied (i.e. depression, selfefficacy, catastrophising and acceptance) only self-efficacy was a significant predictor of disability (beta weight = -.51, t = -8.23, p < 0.001). This is particularly significant as this relationship held even when the effects of education level and pain intensity were controlled for. This finding has been reported elsewhere with some variations. Some studies have reported that self-efficacy is a better predictor of disability than pain intensity (i.e. Nicholas and Asghari, 2006; Jensen et al, 1994), while others found that self-efficacy is a poorer predictor of disability than pain intensity (Arnstein et al. 1999; Geisser et al. 2003), or not even correlated with disability (Altmaier et al. 1993). These 192 differences on findings may be due to the use of different self-efficacy measures, as well as sample differences. In relation to differences in measures, the PSEQ developed by Nicholas (1989); ask respondents if they can do things despite pain, while other self-efficacy measures do not specify that people should consider their pain while evaluating their selfefficacy. This aspect could make a significant difference when evaluating one’s efficacy to a specific situation (i.e. coping with chronic pain). The finding that self-efficacy is a stronger predictor of disability than other cognitive and mood factors has also been reported in the literature (Arnstein 2000; Asghari and Nicholas 2001; Nicholas and Asghari 2006; Turner et al. 2005). The finding of this study is in accordance with Bandura’s theory (1977) that proposes self-efficacy beliefs and appraisal are one of the best predictors of behaviour, and may also influence the development of coping strategies. The contribution of self-efficacy to disability is also in accordance with coping strategies models (Lazarus and Folkman, 1984 and Lazarus, 1993) who recognised that coping strategies are a function of several factors, such as values, commitments, appraisals, beliefs, personality, and other aspects. Therefore, patients with low self-efficacy may have ineffective coping strategies that may contribute to disability. Based on the social learning theory (Bandura, 1977) and on models of coping strategies (Lazarus and Folkman, 1984) it can be hypothesised that the amount of resources people believe they have is a central influence on how people cope with chronic pain. Thus, the belief that one can cope with chronic pain or engage in activities despite pain 193 would contribute to efforts to do so and thus to decrease disability. The corollary is that those whose pain self-efficacy beliefs are weak are less likely to try active coping strategies or to engage in functional activities. In turn, it also means that interventions, like cognitive-behavioural therapy (CBT) pain management programs, should explore ways of enhancing self-efficacy beliefs in participating patients as a key element in reducing disability levels. There is strong evidence that disability due to chronic pain is a composite of several factors, with catastrophising, depression and acceptance playing important roles (e.g. Turner, 2000; Turk, 2000; McCracken and Eccleston, 2003). However, in this study the contribution of these other variables to disability, such as catastrophising (e.g. Sullivan et al.), acceptance (McCracken et al.) and depression (e.g. Pincus and Williams, 1999) were not confirmed. The low contribution of catastrophising to disability (p = 0.19), could be due to some shared variance with other variables (e.g. depression), but multicollinearity did not seem to be a problem in the data set. To examine this aspect, a post hoc analysis was conducted entering only variables that attained significant levels (i.e. education and pain site), self-efficacy and catastrophising. Even without entering depression, activity engagement and pain willingness in the regression analysis, catastrophising did not contribute to disability (beta weight = .11, t = 2.40, p = 0.02) at the adjusted p=0.001. This finding confirms that this result was not obtained by chance; supporting the finding that 194 self-efficacy is a better predictor of physical disability than catastrophising. Nicholas and Asghari (2006) have reported a low contribution of catastrophising to disability when compared to self-efficacy. Turner et al. (2000) also found that catastrophising was a better predictor of depression than of disability. They suggested that catastrophising may have an indirect effect on disability via to effects on depression. But in the present study neither depression nor catastrophising contributed to on disability, especially relative to self-efficacy it is possible that differences in the sample studied (e.g. education, employment and depression) could explain the different findings. In sum, although, some studies have found that catastrophising predicts physical disability (e.g. Sullivan et al., 2005) others have not (Nicholas and Asghari, 2006). The different findings suggest that it is not catastrophising alone that determines this relationship. What is clear in the present study is that catastrophising does not predict disability in this sample of Brazilian chronic pain patients. The failure of the pain acceptance scales (i.e. activity engagement and pain willingness) to contribute to the prediction of disability when self-efficacy, catastrophising and depression are taken into account conflicts with McCracken et al’s. findings (1999 and 2004). Acceptance is a novel concept in this field and although promising needs more conclusive evidence. The variation in findings across studies suggests the role of activity engagement and pain willingness is not robust. Perhaps significantly, it is notable that few studies have investigated the role of 195 acceptance on disability after accounting for the effects of other variables like self-efficacy, catastrophising and fear-avoidance beliefs. Nicholas and Asghari (2006) found that when controlling for age, pain intensity, fear of movement, catastrophising and depression, both scales of the acceptance questionnaire did not predict disability, while self-efficacy did predict disability. Viane et al. (2003) also found no relationship between acceptance and physical disability. Results from the present study are more in accordance with these last studies that suggest acceptance does not appears to be a strong predictor of disability. However, the non-contribution of acceptance to disability could also be due to same shared variance among variables, especially self-efficacy. From a theoretical perspective, as suggested by Nicholas and Asghari (2006), at face value the concept of self-efficacy and acceptance appear to be quite similar. This is partially confirmed by the moderate correlation between the pain self-efficacy questionnaire and the activity engagement scale of the acceptance questionnaire (0.67) reported in the first study. Even so, this relationship is not so high to make either redundant. Activity engagement is described by McCracken et al. as “the pursuit of life activities in a normal manner even while pain is being experienced” (2004, p.164), while, self-efficacy has been described by Nicholas (2007) as “a belief in one’s ability to engage in normal activities despite pain” (p. 160). However, McCracken et al. (2004) proposed that a key difference between these two concepts lie in the fact that acceptance does not include judging the value of being in pain or the ability to 196 manage it. Nicholas and Asghari (2006) also point out that the PSEQ focuses more on functioning independently of pain, while the acceptance questionnaire appears to focus on willingness to live with pain without the need to control it. Based on these definitions, arguments and evidence presented by the authors of the scales, it appears that the most important difference between these measures is that acceptance focuses more on an attitude towards living with pain, while self-efficacy intends to examine beliefs about functioning while in pain. Acceptance seems to be an attitude with emotional bases, while self-efficacy seems to be a belief related more to behaviours than emotions. Thus, it seems that from a conceptual perspective these constructs could be defined differently and maybe the problem of their overlap is related to their definition or to the lack of psychometric properties of the acceptance measure (e.g. findings of study one). 4.7.2 Contribution of socio-demographic, clinical and psychological variables to depression The finding that work status, pain duration, pain intensity, disability, self-efficacy, activity engagement, pain willingness and catastrophising were significantly correlated with depression is in general in conformity with the literature (Campbell et al. 2003; McCracken and Eccleston 2006; Nicholas 2007; Sullivan et al. 2001b). Again, the significance of these results is that for the first time, similar relationships 197 between these variables to those found in high income western countries, have been found in a Brazilian chronic pain patient sample. 4.7.2.1 Contribution of socio-demographic and clinical variables to depression Socio-demographic and clinical variables were not found to be significant predictors of depression (at p=0.001) in a multiple regression analysis. Pain intensity has been frequently reported as a predictor and/or mediator of depression (Jensen et al., 1994; Pincus and Williams, 1999; Arnstein et al., 1999). However, as suggested by Turk et al. (1995) and Arnstein et al. (1999), it seems that pain intensity is not directly related to depression, but is indirectly related to depressive symptomatology through other mediating factors (i.e. catastrophising). The lack of contribution by pain intensity to depression found in this study is in accordance with the findings of Turk et al. (1995) and other researchers (e.g. Pincus et al., 2004). Results of the first study found a significant difference between the working and the not working group on depression. It is likely that work status and depression would have a bidirectional relationship. It has been known that unemployment is a risk factor for depression and equally, that depression is a risk factor for poor outcomes in rehabilitation (Haythornthwaite et al. 1991). 198 The finding that disability was not a significant predictor of depression is interesting given the relationship of work status and depression. This finding has been reported by other studies (Arnstein et al., 1999; McCracken and Eccleston, 2005), and it suggests that work status has more impact on a person’s psychological state than activity limitations by themselves. It points to the view that work represents more than a functional activity and implies that it provides important value to a person’s life (e.g. Morley et al., 2005). Having chronic pain in addition to being unemployed, may represent an additional risk for psychological problems. The findings of Study One have shown that on nearly all the psychometric measures there were significant differences between the working and not working group. However, further exploration of this issue is beyond the scope of this study. The finding that age was not correlated significantly with depression in this chronic pain sample is consistent with previous findings reported by a number of researchers (Nicholas and Asghari, 2006; McCracken and Eccleston; 2005; Viane et al; 2003). Interestingly, there is a higher prevalence of depression in elderly people (Campbell et al. 2003), but in older people with chronic pain it seems that relatively to younger people with chronic pain, those over 60 years of age seem slightly less depressed. It is not clear why this is the case, but it may be that older people might accept pain more than younger people, and thus become less depressed. 199 The finding that that number of pain sites was not predictive of depression but was of disability is puzzling, especially given the significant differences in depression between those with one main pain site and those with two or more (p=0.001). What the MHRA finding suggests is that it is not number of site, per se, that is the determining factor of the difference in depression, but some other factor(s) that are related to number of pain sites. This issue is considered further in the discussion. The finding that pain duration is not a significant predictor of depression has also been reported in the literature (Von Korff et al. 1992). In sum, the findings of this study in relation to the contribution of socio-demographic and clinical variables to depression in this sample appear generally consistent with the published international literature. 4.7.2.2 Contribution of psychological variables to depression In the second step of the regression analysis to examine predictors of depression, after controlling for gender, pain duration, pain intensity and number of pain sites, measures of disability, activity engagement, pain willingness, catastrophising and self-efficacy were entered in the equation. These psychological variables were found to account for an additional 30% of the variance in depression. However, of these variables only catastrophising made a significant contribution to the prediction of depression (beta weight = .45, p < 0.001). 200 The finding that disability, pain self-efficacy, activity engagement, pain willingness, and catastrophising were significantly correlated with depression is consistent with the published literature (e.g. Nicholas and Asghari 2006, Arnstein et al., 1999, McCracken and Eccleston, 2003). Among these variables, catastrophising had the highest correlation with depression, which has also been frequently reported in the literature (e.g. Sullivan et al., 1990, 2005; Nicholas and Asghari, 2006). The contribution of all these variables to depression when analysed together has received little attention in the literature, but the finding that of these variables, catastrophising is the only significant predictor is consistent with previous findings and explains its importance in the assessment of adjustment to chronic pain (e.g. Sullivan et al., 2005; Turner et al., 2000). The finding that disability did not predict depression when selfefficacy beliefs are taken into account has also been reported by Nicholas and Asghari (2006) and Arnstein et al. (1999). When these findings are considered alongside those from the present study, they suggest that cognitive variables are more predictive of depression in chronic pain patients than disability (activity limitation). This conclusion is also consistent with Turk’s diathesis-stress model of chronic pain (2002). The finding that acceptance (activity engagement and pain willingness scales) did not predict depression even at less stringent higher p levels (e.g. p ≤ 0.01) diverges from other findings reported in the literature (McCracken and Eccleston 2003; McCracken and Eccleston 2005; McCracken and Eccleston 2006; McCracken et al. 2004b; Nicholas 201 and Asghari 2006). However, except for the Nicholas and Asghari study the other studies did not include catastrophising in their analyses. These findings indicate that catastrophising makes a greater contribution to depression than acceptance. Nicholas and Asghari (2006) found that only activity engagement was predictive of depression. In contrast, McCracken et al. (2004) found that both CPAQ scales predicted depression. Subsequent studies by McCracken’s group have found that sometimes one and sometimes the other subscale is a better predictor of depression (McCracken and Eccleston, 2005, 2006). The reasons for these differing findings are unclear, but may be related to different samples and different cultures. McCracken’s studies were conducted with Americans and British patients, while Nicholas and Asghari used an Australian sample, and the present study a Brazilian sample. Another issue that may need to be considered in relation to acceptance is the capacity of the CPAQ to adequately capture it. As noted in Study One, only the activity engagement scale was found to have adequate psychometric properties. This is consistent with concerns raised about the CPAQ (e.g. Nicholas and Asghari, 2006). Regarding the contribution of self-efficacy to depression, in the present study pain self-efficacy beliefs did not predict depression even at less stringent p levels (i.e. p ≤ 0.01). This finding diverges from those of Arnstein et al. (1999) and Nicholas and Asghari (2006). However, these studies reported only a small contribution by self-efficacy to depression 202 (6% of variance in the first study, and a beta weight of -.16 at p=0.01 in the second study). On the other hand, the finding that self-efficacy is an important predictor of disability may indicate that this variable could contribute to depression indirectly, but this is only speculation and would have to be tested. However, this hypothesis is in accordance with the nature of the self-efficacy concept proposed by Bandura (1987) and supported by Nicholas (2007). According to this concept it could be expected that pain related self-efficacy beliefs would largely influence the efforts people in pain would put into doing tasks or carrying on their normal life, which might affect indirectly their mood. However, as discussed earlier it seems that cognitions such as those covered by catastrophising are stronger predictor of mood disturbance in chronic pain patients. In that respect, the findings with self-efficacy are consistent with the disability and catastrophising findings. When examining the contribution of catastrophising to depression, the findings of this study suggests that among all psychological variables studied, catastrophising is the most important predictor of depression, and even when accounting for physical disability measured by the RMDQ. In the literature, catastrophising has been frequently reported as a superior predictor of depression than other beliefs and coping responses (Sullivan and D'Eon 1990; Turner et al. 2000), than acceptance (Nicholas and Asghari 2006; Viane et al. 2003), and than self-efficacy and fearavoidance (Nicholas and Asghari 2006). 203 The finding of this study regarding the contribution of catastrophising to depression supported by the literature could indicate that the degree to which a patient experience depression is largely influenced by other factors than pain intensity, physical disability, coping and self-efficacy. As suggested by the cognitive and behavioural model (Beck 1976; Sullivan and D'Eon 1990), cognitions (specifically catastrophising) strongly mediate how people interpret the nature of their reality. Thus, being depressed in the context of having chronic pain has not only to do with being in pain or disabled, but more to do with cognitive-evaluative processes. At times the large contribution of catastrophising to depression has been thought to be due to some shared variance with depression (Sulliva and D’Eon, 1990). However, several studies have shown that this constructs are distinct although with some similar elements (Beck 1976; Jones et al. 2003; Sullivan et al. 2005). According to cognitive models, both constructs have a helplessness component, but catastrophising is marked more by rumination and magnification components. Sullivan et al. (2001) suggested that catastrophising could contribute to depression through biasing the information process, heightening the pain experience and thus influencing emotional functioning and the development of depression. Jones et al. (2003) suggested that catastrophising should not be conceptualised merely as a cognitive coping construct, but as a construct with both cognitive and affective components. Severeijnes et al. (2005) found that the role of catastrophising in different populations (general population and 204 inpatients) seemed to vary in nature. Overall, while research into the ways in which catastrophising might operate with people in chronic pain, the results of the present study in relation to catastrophising are clearly consistent with those found in a range of countries and cultures. This suggests that interventions for chronic pain that are directed at these different psychological factors would warrant a trial in Brazilian chronic pain patients. 4.7.3 Contribution of socio-demographic, clinical and psychological variables to pain intensity As noted before, only gender and pain self-efficacy had a significant contribution to pain intensity at the adjusted p level (p ≤ 0.007), however catastrophising would be considered a significant contributor of pain intensity at a less stringent p level (p = 0.01). However, the impact of these variables is still quite small. The first set of predictors (gender) explained only 2% of the variance in pain intensity. The second group of cognitive and mood variables explained an additional 13% of the variance, with only self-efficacy (beta weight= .28, p= -3.82, p<0.001) making a significant contribution to predict pain intensity. 205 4.7.3.1 Contribution of socio-demographic and clinical variables to pain intensity The finding that there was a gender difference in pain intensity scores and that gender is correlated and contributes to pain intensity (beta weight = .16, t= 2.91, p=0.004) is in accordance with other published studies in both experimental pain studies (Sheffield et al. 2000) and clinical results (Turp et al. 1997), Although, there is no consensus on what the possible mechanisms involved in these differences, some have suggested possible physiological and cultural differences may be involved (Edwards et al. 2001a). From a cultural perspective, there is evidence that in some cultures it is more acceptable for women to express pain behaviour than men and thus this could affect pain report (Hobara 2005). Furthermore, gender differences have been reported on measures of catastrophising and depression (Keefe et al., 2001), which could effect indirectly the relationship between gender and pain intensity. However, in this series of studies there were no differences on mean scores between males and females on depression and catastrophising. Thus, the small contribution of gender to pain intensity in the studied population may be explained by cultural factors, but this is only a speculation. 206 4.7.3.2 Contribution of psychological variables to pain intensity The finding that pain willingness, activity engagement and depression did not contribute significantly to pain intensity has been reported in the literature, but not consistently (Nicholas and Asghari 2006; Viane et al. 2003). Based on the concept of acceptance described by Hayes et al. (1999) and McCracken et al. (2004), it could be hypothesised that accepting any given situation would lessen the distress or unpleasantness of it. Thus, it could be expected that an accepting attitude would contribute to less unpleasantness, reflected in the affective component of pain (e.g. the affective dimension of pain captured by the McGill Pain Questionnaire). However, the influence of acceptance on pain intensity varies considerably in the literature. Acceptance has been reported to predict pain (p=0.05) by McCracken and Eccleston (2003), however that was without controlling for demographic, clinic and other psychological variables except for coping strategies. McCracken et al. (2004) and McCracken and Eccleston (2006) found that only the pain willingness scale is a significant predictor of pain intensity (p=0.001). On the other hand, Nicholas and Asghari (2006) reported no contribution of acceptance to pain intensity (at a p > 0.01). The finding that depression did not contribute to pain intensity is also in accordance with some studies. A review by Dickens et al. (2003) found that depressed patients are less likely to perceive a noxious stimulus as being painful than non-depressed patients. Sullivan et al. 207 (2001) reported that depression does not predict pain intensity when catastrophising is taken into account, but could be associated with the experience of heightened pain and emotional distress in response to pain. Taken together, these findings may suggest that depression does not play an important role on pain intensity. Among the psychological variables self-efficacy had the strongest contribution to pain intensity (beta weight= -.28, t = -3.82, p<0.001). Nicholas and Asghari (2006) and Altmaier et al. (1993) also have found that self-efficacy predicts pain intensity and plays an important role as a mediator between pain intensity and disability, as described previously. The results of this study support this evidence in this specific population. It could be hypothesised that patients with lower self-efficacy could feel more vulnerable to their pain, due to the lack of perceived resources to cope with it. On the other hand, higher levels of pain would demand more self-resources, which could be perceived by the patient as beyond their capacity. The measure of self-efficacy used here does ask respondents to take pain into account and accordingly the findings might be interpreted in that light. However, as Nicholas (2007) reported, PSEQ scores appear to be not only influenced by pain severity and other factors such as the belief in one’s ability to perform a task, would also be expected to play a role. Catastrophising did not predict pain intensity in the present study at the adjusted p level, but it was significant at the less stringent p level of 0.01 (beta weight= .17, t= 2.47). Furthermore, a post hoc analysis found that (after controlling for gender) the unique contribution of the 208 two catastrophising factors explained 9% of the variance for pain intensity, but only factor 1, named rumination was significant (beta weight =.29, t=4.25, p < 0.001). The findings in relation to catastrophising and the rumination factor are in accordance with the findings from experimental studies (e.g. Sullivan et al., 1995) and clinical studies (e.g. Michael and Burns, 2004; Nicholas and Asghari, 2006). Sullivan et al. (2005) in a study with neuropathic patients found that the rumination scale of the PCS (which has similar item contents to factor one of the PRSS catastrophising scale) accounted for a significant proportion of the variance in affective pain. However, in other studies with subjects with longer pain duration and neuropathic pain the helplessness factor made a greater contribution to pain intensity than the other factors. Sullivan et al. (2005) suggested that the nature and duration of pain seemed to evoke different catastrophic dimensions. In the present study, the contribution of both factors (rumination and helplessness) to depression, and the contribution of catastrophising and the rumination factor to the prediction of pain intensity suggest that different aspects of catastrophising differently affect depression and pain intensity. In this case, helplessness seemed to play a more dominant role in depression and rumination seems to contribute more to pain intensity. This is in accordance with evidence gathered by Sullivan et al. (2001, 2005), who suggested that a helpless orientation might compromise efforts to cope with pain and mood, while rumination would account for an excessive focus on the noxious stimulus. 209 4.7.4 Examining risk factors for work status In this sample of Brazilian chronic pain patients, of whom 41% were unemployed due to pain, educational level, age, physical disability and self-efficacy were found to be significant predictors of unemployment. In contrast, of the variables measured, pain intensity, pain site, pain duration, gender, depression, acceptance and catastrophising were not found to be risk factors for unemployment in this sample. Educational level had the strongest effect on unemployment due to pain (odds ratio=3.49 p=0.001), followed by physical disability (odds ratio=2.75 p=0.001), age (odds ratio=0.39 p=0.004)and self-efficacy (odds ratio=2.52 p=0.04). There is good evidence from elsewhere that individual and workplace or system related factors play an important role in disability and return to work (Marhold et al. 2002; Sullivan et al. 2005; Teasell and Bombardier 2001). A number of studies have also suggested that perceptions and beliefs about work and returning to work may play an important role on recovery and disability (Marhold et al. 2002; Schultz et al. 2004; Sullivan et al. 2005). Marhold er al. (2002) found that perceived prognosis of return to work, social support at work, physical workload and harmfulness, pain intensity and depression (measured by the obstacles to return to work questionnaire) were significant predictors of sick leave after 9 months. Blyth et al. (2005), in a population based study, found that people not working due to pain reported more use of 210 passive coping strategies. Other cross-sectional studies have also found that catastrophising is a significant predictor of more prolonged work absence in injured workers (Sullivan et al. 1998; Sullivan and Stanish 2003). Ghelfold et al. (2005) found that pain-related fears were significant determinants of the inability to work in people with back pain. Depression has also been associated with greater occupational disability and is involved in transition from acute to chronic pain (Sullivan et al. 1992; Sullivan et al. 1998). Schultz et al.’s (2004) findings suggest that perception of health change and expectations of recovery were significant predictors of return to work. Other studies of predictors for return to work after treatment, have found that age, marital status, education and decreased length of unemployment were important predictors of return to work (Tan et al. 1997). More recently, Vowles et al. (2004) found that age, pain duration and levels of reported disability were predictive of work status. The finding that age, educational level and disability were risk factors for unemployment in this Brazilian sample with chronic pain is in accordance with findings from other countries (e.g. Vowles et al., 2004; Tan et al., 1997;). However, it is important to recognise that work status was not one of the main outcome variables investigated in this study. Thus this study did not explore a range of other dimensions that have also been related to return to work for injured workers. This applies specifically to work related factors, such as suitable work availability, employer-related factor and compensation (e.g. Teasell and Bombardier, 2001; Sullivan et al., 211 2005). The present study focused only on individual factors, and intended to interpret unemployment due to pain as an indicator of disability. In considering why educational level was the most important risk factor for this sample, it may be relevant to note that in a developing country like Brazil, employment is likely to be influenced by level of education. In this country interventions aiming to help chronic pain patients to return to work would have to address this aspect as well (e.g. training needs). This is not peculiar to Brazil, however, as studies in high income countries have reported similar findings (Schultz et al. 2004). The findings of this study that educational level was a predictor of disability and the highest risk factor to unemployment in patients with chronic pain are also often reported in the international literature (e.g. Hagen et al., 2006; Tan et al., 1997). There are also a few Brazilian studies reporting an association between low back pain and socioeconomic level (da Silva et al. 2004; Pimenta 2001). However, even in the international literature these relationships are poorly understood. There are diverging models and evidence regarding the relationship between educational level and work status in the general population and in chronic pain patients. The human capital theory outlines that educational level mediates social mobility and work conditions (Henriques 2000). However, recent studies (Pochmann 2004) conducted in Brazil with the general population have found that in developing countries characterised by low economic growth, lower investment in technology and worsening work environment conditions (i.e. Brazil), increasing level of education has not contributed 212 to unemployment. On the other hand, it appears that people with higher levels of education that are employed do earn higher salaries than those with lower levels of education. In the chronic pain field there is some evidence of the relationship between educational level and work status but the underlying mechanisms are not clear. Dionne et al. (2001) and Hagen et al. (2006) suggest that this relationship could be related to the fact that people with lower levels of education are more likely to work in physical and psychological demanding jobs. Therefore, having lower levels of education would increase the likelihood of having a job that is physically demanding, thus not being physically fit would increase the likelihood of not being able to work in this population. Furthermore, well-educated people are more likely to have healthier life styles than those with lower education (e.g. obesity, sedentary) (Leboeuf-Yde 2000). This could also explain the contribution of educational level on disability, which could indirectly contribute to unemployment. Evidence of the relationship between educational level, disability and work status in chronic pain patients is scarce in the Brazilian literature. But it might be expected that this relationship would occur in developing countries to a higher degree than in developed countries due to more severe socio-economic conditions that may affect chronic pain patients even more. Therefore, it could be hypothesised that a combination of factors (i.e. having low education and chronic pain) would 213 increase the likelihood of being unemployed in a developing country more than in a developed country. However this is only speculation, suggesting further research. Other studies with working populations have found that gender (female) and lower socio-economic level were predictors of disability due to chronic pain (Enthoven et al. 2006; Jablonska et al. 2006). Findings from this study could not examine the contribution of socio-economic level to unemployment. However, in the studied sample, gender was not associated with unemployment. It is possible that this might reflect employment opportunities in Brazil being different for men and women. Results from this sample also suggest that pain intensity did not contribute to unemployment. Regarding this issue there is mixed evidence in the literature. Shaw et al. (2005) investigated a group of patients attending occupational health clinics and found that pain level and mood were stronger predictors of return to work than health history or physical findings. On the other hand, there is evidence that pain intensity was an important predictor of disability and unemployment due to pain (Enthoven et al. 2006; Jablonska et al. 2006). As noted before, the biopsychosocial model of pain holds that pain is a multidimensional phenomenon and that several factors interact in shaping pain experiences. Therefore in different samples and different cultures it is expected that the relationship between chronic pain and disability will be slightly different but will be broadly similar. Physical disability (measured by the RMDQ) was a significant risk factor for unemployment (odds ratio=2.75). It could be expected that 214 patients with high levels of disability (scores higher than the third quartile) would be on sick leave, retired or unemployed. Although, there are a number of reasons for not returning to work, the contribution of disability to not returning to work is frequently reported in the literature (Enthoven et al. 2006; Vowles et al. 2004). Thus the findings of the present study would seem broadly consistent with those from other studies. While previous studies have found that depression (Cohen et al. 2000; Robbins et al. 1996; Vowles et al. 2004), anxiety and lower levels of self-efficacy have been associated with unemployment due to pain (Robbins et al. 1996), this study found that self-efficacy was more important than these other psychological variables. This finding is interesting and may indicate differences in work environment and social security contingencies between Brazil and other more developed countries. As described in the results, although self-efficacy was a risk factor for unemployment in this Brazilian sample with chronic pain, its contribution was smaller than the contribution of disability and educational level. Based on this, it could be hypothesised that socio-economic and clinical factors play an important role in work status, which could be also influenced by the person’s belief in their ability to function despite pain. It seems that self-efficacy might partially explain how some people in pain can carry on with their working activities; especially in a developing country were socio-economic factors can be more severe than 215 in a developed country, and that there is usually less government or insurance support for the unemployment in the developing countries. 216 4.7.5 Summary of discussion and implications As predicted, this study found that a number of psychosocial factors might contribute to disability, work status, emotional adjustment and pain intensity in patients with chronic pain. These findings are clearly supportive of a biopsychosocial perspective of chronic pain and indicate that the model applies to Brazilian chronic pain patients as well as to those from other countries with more developed economies and different cultures. The results show that in this population, socio-demographic factors (i.e. educational level and gender) can interact with clinical variables (i.e. pain site) and contribute to disability (i.e. reported physical disability and work status). Furthermore, cognitions (i.e. catastrophising and selfefficacy) also seem to have an important contribution to pain intensity, disability, work status and emotional adjustment. Biological factors (e.g. pathophysiology) were not assessed in this study, but the strength of the relationships found that psychological and demographic factors indicate that regardless of pathophysiology, they make an important contribution to the impact of chronic pain as outlined by the biopsychosocial model. These findings also partially confirmed the hypotheses of this study that some cognitions and mood would contribute to disability, that some cognitions and disability would contribute to depression and that some socio-demographic and psychological factors would contribute to work status. 217 From a clinical perspective the findings of this study suggest that interventions based in these concepts can be expected to be relevant to the Brazilian chronic pain population. The next study will examine more closely the relationships reported in this study in a matched Australian sample with chronic pain, which will be compared to the findings reported above. 218 CHAPTER FIVE Study Three: A cross-cultural comparison of the contribution of cognitions to disability and depression: A comparison between an Australian and a Brazilian sample with chronic pain. 5.1 Introduction In this series of studies the results of Study Two have supported some of the basic tenets of the biopsychosocial model of pain; namely that cognitive factors influence adjustment to chronic pain. Among several psychosocial factors, it is also expected that cultural factors may influence pain perception and responses to it, but this feature has not been yet considered in this series of studies. Cultural factors are thought to influence how a person perceives, experiences and responds to pain. Cultural factors may include: values, beliefs, norms and social practices (Magnusson and Fennell 2005). Although there are mixed findings and a lack of conclusive evidence regarding the influence of cultural factors on the impact of chronic pain on individuals, most findings support the existence of differences among ethnic groups on pain perception (Bates et al. 1993; Moore and Brodsgaard 1999). Mechanisms involved in the relationship between culture and pain are not clear and are likely to be complex with a lot of variation even within a culture (Skevington, 1995; Dimsdale, 2000). Recent findings also suggest that ethnic groups differ regarding emotional responses to chronic pain and present different coping 219 strategies (Edwards et al. 2005; Hastie et al. 2004; Riley et al. 2002). However, a number of studies have shown that when ethnic groups are matched on confounding variables (i.e. income or social classes) these differences are relatively small (Edwards et al. 2005; Portenoy et al. 2004). From a clinical perspective, responses to chronic pain seem to be more important than variance in noxious stimulus perception (Tait and Chibnall 2005). It has been argued that ethnicity influences the course of chronic pain and its treatment (Dimsdale 2000; Tait and Chibnall 2005). Recently, studies have focused on clinical implications regarding ethnic differences on pain responses (i.e. treatment disparities, treatment engagement, tailoring treatments according to individual or group differences) (Edwards et al. 2005). Examining ethnic differences may also be important from a theoretical perspective. Since current pain models and much of the evidence have largely been produced in economically advanced western cultures (mainly European and North American). It is important to understand to what extent these findings can be generalised to other cultures. In this study we are interested in determining if the psychosocial factors described in the international pain literature have a similar role in a Brazilian chronic pain population. While a number of studies have confirmed similar pain prevalence rates (Macfarlane 2005), the role of psychosocial factors on pain and disability (Gureje et al. 1998), as well the efficacy of multidisciplinary CBT pain management interventions in different countries (Blyth et al. 220 2005), there is relatively little cross-cultural research in this field. In particular few studies have compared very distinct populations in different cultures regarding the contribution of psychosocial factors to disability. The results presented in Study Two indicated broad similarities in relationship between psychosocial variables and pain dimensions between Brazilian chronic pain patients and findings published in the pain literature. However, that study did not compare the Brazilian sample with a matched sample of chronic pain patients from a more developed and English speaking country. So, the present study was intended to test the generalizability of the previous findings with a matched comparison sample of chronic pain patients in Australia. If the previous findings are confirmed it would further strength the generalizability of those findings. 5.2 Socio-demographic characteristics of Brazil and Australia 5.2.1 Brazil According to the Brazilian Bureau of Statistics (Instituto Brasileiro de Geografia e Estatística - IBGE 2000), the Brazilian population is 186.808.564, distributed in an area of 8.514.876 km2. Men account for 49.22% of the population while women are 50.78%. Age distribution is 014 years (29.60%), 15-64 years (64.55%) and over 65 years (5.85%). Around 81% of the Brazilian population live in urban areas, while 18% live in rural areas. Expectancy of life is 71.59 years. Portuguese is the 221 national language. The illiteracy rate is 11.6%, however most of the population has 4 to 7 years of education. The Brazilian Gross Domestic Product (GDP) is US$ 602 billion, with a per capita income of capita US$ 8.017. However, Brazil has the second worst income distribution in the world and unemployment rate is 9.1% (Instituto Brasileiro de Geografia e Estatística - IBGE 2000). Nearly 56% of the working force earn up to US$ 350.00 a month, 21% earn between US$ 350.00 and US$ 800.00, around 17% earn between US$ 800.00 and 1700.00 a month, and only less than 5% of the working force earn more than US$ 1700.00 a month. The Portuguese started the colonisation of Brazil in 1500 and although the Brazilian population is a blend of Portuguese, Indians (aborigines) and Africans, there is a large contribution of several ethnicities through immigration cycles. Most of immigrants came to Brazil between 1884 and 1959 (approximately 4 million), and originally were from Portugal, Italy, German, Spain, Lebanon and Japan (in order of magnitude). According to the last census, 53.7% of the population is White, 39.5% are Brown, 6.2% are Black, 0.4% are Asians and 0.4% are Indians. Most of the Brazilian population is Christian and only 7% report to have no religion (Instituto Brasileiro de Geografia e Estatistica - IBGE 2000). In general Brazilians have strong religious beliefs and religion plays a big role in the Brazilian culture. Brazil is a very diverse culture and, in general, well integrated. The Health system in Brazil is publicly-funded and according to the constitution every Brazilian should have free access to health service and 222 medication. The quality of the services varies from one region to another, but long waiting periods can occur for some specialities (e.g. 6 months or more). There are discrepancies on access to the health system related to socio-economic levels. In the last decade, there has been an increase in the private sector investment in this area. Therefore, it is increasingly common for middle class people to have a private health plan and access to better health services. As in most health systems, treatment is a major priority, while educative interventions and preventive approaches are not as well supported. There is a small number of pain clinics in the public sector and less than a dozen in the private sector. Furthermore, general epidemiological data for chronic pain is scarce in Brazil, but it could be assumed to be similar to that obtained in other countries (Gureje et al. 1998). 5.5.2 Australia The European colonisation of Australia followed the English discovery of the continent in 1778. There have been several immigration waves, firstly lead by the British and Irish settlers (often convicts being sent for the rest of their lives), followed by other European countries especially after World War II (i.e. eastern and southern European countries), Asian, Middle Eastern countries and lately African countries. Around 2.5 million immigrants arrived in Australia from 1985 to 2000, making it a very multicultural society (Australian Bureau of Statistics. 223 2004). Due to its new immigration waves, a number of immigrants are still in an acculturation process. Most of the Australians report to have a religion (i.e. Anglican, catholic or other), but 15% have no religion and nearly 12% have not reported having a religion or responded wrongly in the Census (Australian Bureau of Statistics 2006). In general, religion does not have a very obvious role in the Australian culture as compared to Brazil. Australia has a population of 20.592.324 people distributed in 7617930 kmh. Men account for 49.8% of the population. Age distribution is 0-14 years (20.70%), 15-64 years (67%) and over 65 years (12.3). Nearly 86% of the Australian population live in urban areas, while 14% live in rural areas. Expectancy of life is 77 years for males and 82 years for females, respectively. Regarding the educational level, 41% are below upper secondary level (leave school at age 15), 30% are above upper secondary, 10% have post-secondary education and 19% reach university. Literacy rate is thought to be nearly 100% (Australian Bureau of Statistics. 2004). The Australian’s GDP is US$ 637 billion, with a per capita income of capita US$ 27.070. Wealth in Australia is more evenly distributed than in most countries and the unemployment rate in 2004 was around 4.9% (Parliamentary Library. 2002). The Health system in Australia is publicly funded, but there is a strong health private sector as well. The government subsidises the private health fees and a number of medications. The quality of the services tend to be better in the cities, but long waiting periods can occur 224 for some specialities (e.g. 6 months or more for some elective procedures). Therefore, it is common to have a health plan to try for faster access to health services. As in most health systems, and similarly to Brazil, treatment is the major priority, while educative interventions and preventive approaches are less emphasised. There are discrepancies in access to the health system related to socio-economic levels. There are a number of pain clinics in public hospitals and some in the private sector as well. Contrary to Brazil, there are some epidemiological data confirming a similar prevalence of chronic pain to that reported by the international literature (e.g. Blyth et al., 2001). To summarise, there are many socio-economic and demographic differences between Australia and Brazil. Although in process of development, Brazil is more characterised by social and economic disparities (moderate level of unemployment, lack of housing, high levels of criminality, but also extremely wealthy families). Due to their colonisation processes and immigration waves, both countries are also culturally diverse. Despite the differences between these two countries, in many areas the health systems of both countries face similar problems. Although less problematic in Australia, in both countries the public health system is struggling, there are difficulties in transcending the curative model, to increase early interventions and to implement more holistic approaches for managing chronic diseases. Beside these, working injury legislation and work place issues can interfere with returning to work in both countries. 225 Despite their differences, people living with chronic pain in these two countries face a number of similar problems which may increase their suffering and create difficulties in their ability to return to normal, daily activities. These include the dominance of the bio-medical model in the health system, delayed interventions for pain, multiple barriers for return to work after being in pain for a number of years, and lack of training for health professionals in the management of chronic pain. Therefore, despite the differences in culture and language, living with a chronic disease (i.e. chronic pain) in a western society even in different countries may be an important homogenising factor. As pain is a universal phenomenon, this feature may in turn make this group of people much more similar than different. But this needs to be tested. 226 5.3 Method 5.3.1 Subjects Data collected from 311 subjects with chronic pain attending pain clinics or similar facilities in public and private services in Brazil were matched with data from a paired population that attended the Pain Management and Research Centre - Royal North Shore Hospital located in Sydney, Australia6. Data from the Pain Management Research Centre (PMRC) was also derived from 311 patients. 5.3.2 Inclusion and exclusion criteria Participants were included or excluded in this study based on criteria described in Study One. In both groups, questionnaires with more than 10% of missing items were not used in the statistical analysis. 5.3.3 Procedure The Brazilian sample was recruited first, as described in Study One. The Australian sample was selected from patients being seen at the PMRC during 2005-2006. The Australian sample was identified by file search, which aimed to match the Brazilian sample according to age and gender. The inclusion criteria for the Australian sample were similar to those of the Brazilian sample, in that informed consent to use their 6 This series of studies had the approval of the Royal North Shore Hospital ethics committee attached on appendix 2. 227 (anonymous) data for the research was required, they had to be able to read English enough to complete the questionnaires, and they had to be presented to the clinic with chronic (more than 3 months) non-cancer pain. Data collected from questionnaires previously completed by patients attending the PMRC were entered in a database for statistical analyses using SPSS 14 for Windows. 5.3.4 Measures As described in Chapter Three data were collected using the following questionnaires: the Roland and Morris Disability Questionnaire (Roland and Morris 1983), the Pain Self-Efficacy Questionnaire - PSEQ (Nicholas, 1989), the PRSS Catastrophising scale (Flor et al., 1993), the Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire - CPAQ (McCracken et al. 2004b), the DASS Depression scale (Lovibond and Lovibond 1993), a socio demographic, and a clinical inventory. All the psychometric properties of these measures have been described in Study One and in the literature, and all have adequate validity and reliability. 5.4 Hypothesis This study is based on the hypothesis that the relationships between pain, self-efficacy beliefs, acceptance, catastrophising, depression and disability should occur in a similar pattern in these two samples. 228 5.5 Aims of study The major aim of this study is to compare the relationships among socio-demographic and clinical variables, self-efficacy beliefs, acceptance, catastrophising, depression, and disability in a Brazilian and an Australian chronic pain population and to examine if these findings are consistent with those reported in the literature. 5.6 Statistical analyses A number of statistical analyses were conducted to examine the data, mainly independent sample t-tests, ANOVA, correlations, hierarchical and logistic regressions (Tabachnick and Fidell, 2001). All the analyses were conducted using the SPSS for Windows version 14.0. Firstly, descriptive statistics from the Australian sample is presented. Second, similarly to Study Two the relationships between socio-demographic, clinical and psychological variables were examined in the Australian sample using correlation coefficients, hierarchical regression and logistic regression analyses. After that the Australian and Brazilian samples were compared. The samples were compared using independent sample t-tests and analyses of variance (ANOVA). Whenever, a t-test was run, Levene’s test was performed to examine equal variances difference between groups, when ANOVA was conducted a Scheffe post hoc tests was run. Findings of the regression and logistic analyses are compared between samples. 229 5.7 Results This section will present the findings regarding the contribution of socio-demographic, clinical and psychological factors to disability and depression within the Australian chronic pain sample. In the discussion these findings will be compared with those from the Brazilian sample (already presented in Study Two) and with the current literature. The distribution of the data collected at the Pain Management and Research Centre were inspected to check for skewed distributions, multicollinearity and singularity. Data were confirmed as normally distributed, correlations between variables were inferior to .90 and there were no redundant variables, therefore no statistical transformations were necessary. Due to the coding system used on data entry some variables were transformed to dummy variables. Work status initially categorised into working, partially working and not working due to pain was coded as 0 (for working and partially working) and 1 (for not working due to pain). Similarly the variable pain site was re-coded; all single pain sites were coded as 0, while pain in two or more sites was coded as 1. Descriptive statistics are presented in Table 1 for socio- demographic and clinical characteristics of the PMRC sample. In table 2 mean scores on the psychological measures are described. In Tables 3 and 4 differences in mean scores associated with work status and educational level were examined using t-tests and analyses of variance (ANOVA) respectively, followed by comments of other differences associated with gender and pain site on a number of measures. 230 Table 5.1 Socio-demographic and clinical characteristics of the Australian sample Socio- demographic and clinical characteristics Australian sample No (%) Age (mean) 49.2 (SD=14.8) Gender Male 83 (26.7) Female 228 (73.3) Marital status Single 74 (23.7) Married 188 (60.5) Separated, divorced or widowed 49 (15.8) Educational Level Less than year 10 77 (25.7) Up to year 12 or technical 146 (45.0) Higher level of education (i.e. university) 88 (29.3) Employment status Working 183 (58.8) Unemployed due to pain 128 (41.2) Pain duration From 3 months up to 1 year 55 (17.8) 1 to 3 years 72 (24.2) 3 to 5 years 72 (24.2) 6 to 9 years 27 (9.1) Up to 10 years 74 (24.7) Pain intensity (mean) 5.8 (SD=2.0) Major pain sites Head, face, neck 27 (8.7) Cervical, shoulders and upper limbs 44 (14.1) Back or spine 31 (10.0) Lower back and lower limbs 46 (14.8) Two or more major sites 107 (34.4) Other 56 (18.0) Most of the participants (60.5%) were married, 45% had up to 12 years of formal education, and nearly 41% of the participants were not working due to pain. Mean pain duration was defined as an ordinal 231 variable, and the majority of participants (58%) had experienced pain for more than 3 years. Mean average pain intensity was rated 5.8 out of 10. Most of the participants had pain in two or more sites (34.4%), followed by pain in lower back and lower limbs (14.8%), cervical, shoulder and upper limbs (14.1%), and back or spine pain (10%). Other pain sites with a smaller prevalence were grouped together (i.e. pelvic, anal, genital, thoracic and abdominal). In the next table, scores on the psychological measures are described. Statistical analyses were run to analyse the distribution and mean on a number of measures (disability, self-efficacy, depression, catastrophising and acceptance) and a number of other variables (e.g. age, pain intensity) in the Australian sample in a similar fashion to that used in Study One. The normal distribution of patients by gender at the PMRC was 57.4% of women, but in order to match the Brazilian sample the ration had to be altered to select proportionally more women, until the PMRC study sample was 73% women. 232 Table 5.2 Descriptive statistics for the Australian sample in all the measures Questionnaires Means (SD) Skewness Kurtosis Score range Roland and Morris Disability Quest. 11.66 (6.62) 0.42 -0.90 0-24 Pain Self-Efficacy Questionnaire 27.57 (13.65) 0.27 -0.63 0-60 DASS - Depression Scale 11.73 (10.44) 1.00 0.40 0-42 2.73 (1.50) -0.06 -0.56 0-5 Chronic Pain Acceptance Quest. 59.65 (17.56) -0.57 0.26 0-120 Activity Engagement Scale 32.67 (12.47) 0.07 -0.21 0-66 Pain Willingness Scale 28.08 (10.80) -0.28 -0.53 0-54 PRSS - Catastrophising Scale The results indicate that the data are normally distributed, with skewness and kurtosis values inferior or nearly 1.0 (Tabachnick and Fidell 2001). Furthermore no clear outliers were identified. Thus, statistical transformation to normalise the data was not necessary. Differences regarding socio-demographic characteristics on mean score are presented on the next tables. 233 Table 5.3 Comparison of scores on questionnaires by educational level up to year 12 Higher level Test of years of or technical of education significance education of education Questionnaires Less than 10 ρ F mean (SD) mean (SD) mean (SD) RMDQ 12.82 (6.09) 11.61 (6.18) 9.71 (5.86) PSEQ 25.29 (14.07) 27.64 (13.37) 28.90 (14.29) 1.46 0.23 Depression Scale 11.72 (10.40) 10.86 (10.16) 10.94 (10.15) 0.19 0.82 2.72 (0.98) 2.69 (1.05) 2.50 (1.02) 1.28 0.27 CPAQ 57.10 (18.17) 60.73 (17.48) 59.46 (16.20) 1.03 0.35 AE Scale 33.07 (12.67) 33.03 (12.08) 32.25 (12.05) 0.13 0.87 PW Scale 24.12 (11.69) 27.71 (11.71) 27.12 (9.63) 2.71 0.07 Catastrophising 5.79 0.003 No significant differences were found between education levels on measures of self-efficacy, depression, catastrophising, activity engagement, pain willingness, and total acceptance. However there was a significant difference on the Roland and Morris Disability Questionnaire. When further analysis (Post Hoc analysis using Scheffe to analyse significance differences with in groups) were conducted, there was only significant differences on the RMDQ mean score between the group with less than 10 years of education and the group with tertiary education (p = 0.004). Participants with lower educational level (up to 10 years) were more disabled than those with tertiary education. Comparison between groups on the basis of work status for each measure is presented below. 234 Table 5.4 Comparison of scores on questionnaires by work status Questionnaires Not working Working due to pain mean (SD) mean (SD) Australian t-test of total sample significance ρ mean (SD) t 11.37 (6.16) 3.81 0.001 RMDQ 12.67 (6.08) 9.81 (5.87) PSEQ 24.00 (13.43) 29.13 (13.15) 27.39 (13.84) -3.06 0.002 Depression Scale 13.64 (10.44) 9.29 (9.51) 11.11 (10.19) 3.45 0.001 Catastrophising 2.87 (0.89) 2.53 (1.07) 2.64 (1.01) 2.76 0.006 CPAQ 58.10 (16.96) 60.86 (16.85) 59.46 (17.10) -1.21 0.19 AE Scale 31.52 (12.02) 33.12 (11.72) 32.81 (12.19) -1.19 0.28 PW Scale 26.52 (11.65) 27.75 (10.54) 26.61 (11.20) -0.62 0.38 N= 257 (only subjects with less than 65 years were selected for analysis) Although, there was no difference between the two groups on pain intensity (t=0.12, p=0.90), the not working due to pain group differ significantly from those working on mean scores on disability, selfefficacy, depression and catastrophising, but not on acceptance and its factors. Scores of the not working group due to pain are lower on selfefficacy, and higher on disability, depression and catastrophising than the participants that have pain but were still working. Other possible differences on score associated with gender and pain site were examined. There were no significant differences on mean scores associated with gender and pain site on all the measures (p≤0.001). In the next table correlations between a number of variables are described followed by regression analyses. 235 Table 5.5 Pearson correlations among socio-demographic, clinical and psychological measures Age Age Pain Pain Disability duration intensity Depression Self-efficacy Catastrophising Activity Pain engagement willingness - Pain duration 0.14 - Pain intensity 0.04 -0.02 - Disability 0.03 -0.06 0.40* - Depression -0.12 -0.02 0.14 0.38* - Self-efficacy 0.20* 0.09 -0.26* -0.53* -0.52* - Catastrophising -0.20* 0.03 0.19* 0.31* 0.45* -0.43* - Activity engagement 0.19* 0.09 -0.13 -0.22* -0.45* 0.67* -0.38* - Pain willingness -0.14 -0.2 -0.19* -0.12* -0.27* 0.11 -0.19* 0.09 Total Acceptance 0.04 0.06 -0.21* -0.23* -0.49* 0.54* -0.39* 0.76* 0.71* * Significant correlations at P = 0.001 236 As expected there were significant correlations among several variables. Most of the significant correlations were generally low in size (less than 0.40) (Rowntree 1981). The correlations between pain intensity and disability (0.40), disability and self-efficacy (-0.58), depression and self-efficacy (-0.52), depression and catastrophising (0.45), depression and activity engagement (-0.45), self-efficacy and activity engagement (0.67), self-efficacy and catastrophising (-0.43), acceptance and depression (-0.49), and acceptance and self-efficacy (0.54) were in the moderate range (higher than 0.40). Some other correlations were close to the moderate range. These included, depression and self-efficacy (-0.38), depression and activity engagement (-0.38) and acceptance and catastrophising (-0.39). As expected there were high correlations between acceptance and its scales. To examine the relationships among three chosen criterion variables (physical disability, depression and pain intensity), sociodemographic and clinic variables and self-efficacy, acceptance of pain (activity engagement and pain willingness scales) and catastrophising, a series of hierarchical multiple regression analyses (MHRA) were conducted. The rationale for the selection of independent variables to be entered in the analyses was significant levels of correlation with the outcomes as established previously using a Bonferroni correction (P= 0.001), significant differences according to t-tests and ANOVAs and clinical importance. In each equation a block of socio-demographic and clinic variables were entered first in order to control for the associations 237 between these variables and the dependent variable, followed by cognitive and mood variables (enter method). In the first MHRA, although depression is one of the studied outcomes, due to its importance in relation to disability, depression was entered in step two as an independent variable with the other cognitive variables (Table 6). In the second MHRA, due to its importance on depression, disability was entered in step two as an independent variable with the other cognitive variables (see Table 7). In Table 8 the contributions of a number of variables to pain intensity are described. In order to control for Type I error, a Bonferroni adjustment was done once more for each group of analyses. Level of significance was adjusted dividing p at 0.05 by the number of variables entered in the regression analyses for each predictor. The adjusted p value for the each group of variables was: disability (0.05/11 = 0.005), depression (0.05/13 = 0.004), and pain intensity (0.05/5 = 0.01). 238 Table 5.6 MHRA predicting disability (Australian sample) Step and predictors Criterion Total R² F df R² Change F change Betaª t variable: Disability Step1: 0.20 21.80** 256 0.20 21.80** Level of education -.06 -1.2 Gender -.01 -0.3 Pain intensity .27 5.3** Step 2: 0.41 22.21** 251 0.21 18.09** Depression .14 2.4 Active engagement .26 3.9** Pain willingness -.02 -0.4 Catastrophising .04 0.7 Self-Efficacy -.51 -6.9** ª Standardised regression coefficient * P ≤ 0.005 ** p ≤ 0.001 Physical disability measured by RMDQ was significantly predicted by a block of variables that included level of education, gender and pain intensity (accounting for 20% of the variance in disability), but of these, only pain intensity contributed significantly to disability. In the second step measures of depression, activity engagement, pain willingness, catastrophising and self-efficacy were entered into the analysis as a block. These variables accounted for an additional 21% of variance in disability, but only self-efficacy and activity engagement were significant predictors of disability (p ≤ 0.001). 239 Table 5.7 MHRA predicting depression (Australian sample) Step and predictors Betaª t Gender -.01 -0.1 Pain site .01 0.3 Pain intensity -.08 -1.4 Pain duration .01 0.1 Criterion Total R² F df R² Change F change 245 0.03 1.89 variable: Depression Step1: 0.03 Step 2: 0.39 1.89 17.35** 240 0.36 28.86** Disability .15 2.2 Active engagement -.18 -2.5 Pain willingness -.19 -3.5** Catastrophising .18 3.1* Self-Efficacy -.26 3.2* ª Standardised regression coefficient * P ≤ 0.004 ** p ≤ 0.001 Although gender, pain site and pain duration were not associated with depression these variables were entered in the analysis to make it more similar to the analysis conducted with the Brazilian sample. Socio-demographic and clinical variables accounted for only 3% of the variance and did not contribute significantly to depression, which confirmed the lack of correlation between gender, pain site, pain duration and pain intensity and depression. 240 After controlling for the variables entered in the first step, disability and cognitive variables were entered in step two. Altogether this group of variables explained an additional 36% of the variance on depression, with pain willingness (beta weight = -.19, t= -3.5 p=0.001), catastrophising (beta weight = .18, t= 3.1 p = 0.002) and pain self-efficacy (beta weight = -.26, t= - 3.2 p = 0.002) predicting depression. Physical disability and activity engagement did not predict depression at a significant level. In the third MHRA (Table 5.8) psychological variables were examined as predictors for pain intensity. A previous MHRA was conducted entering gender in the equation, which did not predict pain intensity (Rh= 0.0) and therefore was taken out of the equation. Table 5.8 MHRA predicting pain intensity (Australian sample) Step and predictors Total R² F df R² Change F change 0.11 6.42** 255 0.11 6.42** Betaª t Criterion variable: Pain intensity Self-Efficacy -.34 -4.0** Pain willingness -.15 -2.4 .12 1.5 -.05 -0.6 .06 -0.9 Active engagement Depression Catastrophising ª Standardised regression coefficient * P ≤ 0.01 ** p ≤ 0.001 Cognitions and mood variables explained 11% of the variance however self-efficacy was the only variable to attain a significant p level. 241 In the next section the results of the logistic regression for work status are presented. For this purpose, correlations between the variables entered in the regression were examined. As reported in Table 5.5 there were no correlations among variables superior to 0.54 (except for the correlations between activity engagement and self-efficacy 0.67, and acceptance and its scales), therefore only total acceptance was kept in the analysis. Correlation coefficients indicated multicollinearity was not a problem. However, as all the variables were coded into dummy variables (into the same way as it was coded in the Brazilian sample) correlations were examined again. All correlations were below 0.30, which confirmed the lack of multicollinearity even when the variables were re-coded. For the logistic regression, all the variables were re-coded as dichotomous. The dependent variable (work status) was coded as follows: Patients who were working or partially working were coded 0, and patients who were not working were coded 1. Since work status is affected by age, only participants who were more than 18 years old and less than 65 years old were selected for the analysis. Thus, the numbers of available subjects decreased to 207. Mean age was used as the cut off point (44). Patients with less than 44 years old were coded 0, while those with more than 44 were coded 1). Pain in any site was coded 0, while 1 was equal to pain in two or more pain sites. Pain duration was coded 0 for patients with pain for up to 5 years, and 1 for patients who were in pain for 6 years or more. Pain intensity cut off point was established at 5. 242 Education was also coded as a dummy variable. Patients with less than 12 years of education or equivalent levels were coded as 1, while subjects with post-school technical education or tertiary were coded as 0. All the disability and psychological measures were coded in the same direction using upper quartile score as a cut off point (i.e. scores lower than the third quartile were coded 0 and the upper quartile scores were coded 1). However, when higher scores on the measure was a positive outcome, it was coded 0 and lower scores were coded 1 (i.e. self-efficacy and acceptance scales). Reduced (i.e. parsimonious models) were produced by a process of backwards elimination of the independent variables. At each stage, the effect of dropping out an independent variable was assessed using the likelihood ratio test at a significant level p=0.05. Odds ratios, beta coefficients and confidence intervals were inspected at each stage to check for possible confounding or effect modification and the goodness of fit of the model. Step 5 (of seven steps) of the regression for work status attained the best goodness of fit. Although not attaining significant p levels, some variables were kept in the regression model due to their contribution with the best fit of the model. In this case if an independent variable with a Pvalue exceeding P=0.05 improved the model fit, it was retained in the reduced model. After observing these criteria and examining other possibilities for coding the variables, the best model was chosen. Similar to the procedure used in Study Two, analyses were run coding questionnaires 243 scores on groups according to percentile, quartiles and mean cut off point, and different age grouping as well. The results found for the bestfit model did not differ significantly from the ones using other forms of categorisation. Table 5.9 Logistic regression analysis for work status with adjusted odds ratio, p values and confidence intervals (Australian sample) Variable Adjusted odds ratio* 95% C.I. P level Pain site (two or more =1) 2.35 1.24 – 4.47 0.009 Age (≥ 44 years =1) 0.38 0.20 – 0.70 0.002 Educational level (‹12 years =1) 1.94 1.06 – 3.56 0.03 Depression – DASS (≥ 8 =1) 2.53 1.24 – 5.17 0.01 Disability (RMDQ) ª (≥ 12 =1) 1.81 0.85 – 3.87 0.12 Catastrophising (PRSS)ª (≥ 2.7 =1) 0.53 0.25 – 1.12 0.09 *Pain intensity, pain duration, gender, self-efficacy and acceptance were also included in the regression ª Retained for improved model fit. In this model, pain site, educational level, age and depression were significant risk factors for work status. These results indicate that participants with pain in two or more sites had 2.35 more chances of being unemployed due to pain than those with pain in only one site. People aged 44 and over have 62% less chance of being unemployed than those in the younger group (less than 44). Patients with lower educational level (having up to 12 years of education) had nearly twice the likelihood of being unemployed (odds ratio=1.94, p=0.04) than those with higher education. Higher depression scores also was a potent risk 244 factor for unemployment due to pain (odds ratio=2.48). Although, catastrophising was kept in the regression due to its contribution for the goodness of fit of the model it did not attained a significant p level (p=0.05). Pain intensity, pain duration, gender, disability, self-efficacy and acceptance did not seem to be risk factors for unemployment for patients with chronic pain in the Australian sample. 245 5.8 Discussion In this section the major findings regarding the Australian and the Brazilian sample will be compared and examined in relation to the existing literature. Table 5.10 Socio-demographic and clinical characteristics of the Australian and Brazilian sample Australian sample Brazilian sample No (%) No. (%) 49.2 (SD=14.8) 48.9 (SD=14.0) 83 (26.7) 81 (26.0) 228 (73.3) 230 (74.0) 74 (23.7) 55 (17.7) 188 (60.5) 200 (64.3) 49 (15.8) 56 (18.0) 77 (25.7) 101 (32.5) 146 (45.0) 89 (28.6) 88 (29.3) 121 (38.9) Working 183 (58.8) 179 (58.9) Unemployed due to pain 128 (41.2) 125 (41.1) 72 (24.2) 87 (28.0) 5.8 (SD:2.0) 6.2 (SD: 2.4) 27 (8.7) 36 (11.6) Cervical, shoulders and upper limbs 44 (14.1) 48 (15.5) Back or spine 31 (10.0) 16 (5.1) Lower back and lower limbs 46 (14.8) 30 (9.6) Demographic and clinical characteristics Age (mean) Gender Male Female Marital status Single Married Separated, divorced or widowed Educational Level Less than year 10 in Australia 4 to 8 years in Brazil Year 10 to 12 or technical studies in Australia 9 to 11 years (and/or technical) in Brazil Higher level of education (ie. university) Employment status Pain duration (Mode was 3 to 5 years) Pain intensity (mean) Major pain sites Head, face, neck Two or more major sites Other 107 (34.4) 140 (45.0) 56 (18.0) 41 (13.2) 246 As expected, due to the matching process, there were no significant differences (p>0.05) on age and gender between these two samples. There was a significant difference in average pain intensity between the samples, but this difference would not be considered clinically significant. Pain duration on both samples also did not differ significantly (p> 0.05). Furthermore, in both samples the majority of participants had pain in two or more sites. Regarding educational level, these two samples had a significant difference on distribution (Chi-square = 111.26 p= 0.001). The Brazilian sample had a higher percentage of participants with 4 to 8 years of education compared to the Australian sample (32.5% and 25.7%), as well as more participants with tertiary education (i.e. universitary or a higher degree) (38.9% versus 29.3%). The Australian sample had a higher percentage of participants with 10 to 12 years of education or technical courses (45% and 28.6%). The differences between samples are probably related to the source of the data. In the Brazilian sample nearly half came from private clinics (47.5%), thus had a higher socio-economic level (compared to the general Brazilian population), while in Australia data were collected in a public hospital (where only a third were privately insured). Although level of education is distributed differently within the samples, this difference is not extreme and should permit further comparisons. Furthermore, the samples were matched on other important variables (i.e. age and gender). 247 Although the two samples were not intended to be representative of pain patients in both countries, these findings suggest that although it could be expected that the Australian and Brazilian sample would present several differences, they are more similar than different. The biggest difference was regarding educational level, which is to be expected. The samples presented several similarities (e.g. higher number of females, similar pain intensity levels, pain duration, pain site, work status). Therefore it can be hypothesised that the differences regarding the relationships among socio-demographic, clinical and psychological variables are more related to cultural differences (e.g. social learning) as proposed by some biopsychosocial models (e.g. Skevington, 1995) than sample differences regarding some variables (i.e. educational level). Although in a lower percentage in the original Australian sample (57% when not matched for gender), the higher prevalence of women in both samples is in accordance with the literature, which suggests that a number of pain syndromes are more common among women and that women seek health care more often than men (Blyth et al. 2001; Gureje et al. 1998). The high prevalence of unemployment associated with chronic pain was very similar in both samples (41%). This finding is also reported in the literature suggesting that chronic pain can have a great social and economic impact (Blyth et al. 2001; Breivik et al. 2006; Raspe et al. 2004; van Leeuwen et al. 2006). Scores of both samples on a number of measures will be described and compared in the next table (Table 5.11). 248 Table 5.11 Comparison between questionnaire means of the Brazilian and Australian samples Questionnaires Brazilian Australian Sample Sample Means (SD) Means (SD) 12.03 (6.21) 11.66 (6.62) 1.27 P = 0.20 -0.34 1.59 Pain Self-Efficacy Questionnaire 34.84 (14.08) 27.57 (13.65) 6.50 P = 0.001 5.07 9.46 DASS - Depression Scale 14.03 (12.02) 11.73 (10.44) 3.84 P = 0.001 1.66 5.14 2.38 (1.38) 2.73 (1.50) -2.70 P = 0.007 -0.45 -0.07 Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire 60.20 (18.11) 59.65 (17.56) -0.66 P = 0.50 -3.72 1.83 Activity Engagement Scale 39.89 (14.07) 32.67 (12.47) 6.45 P = 0.001 4.81 9.02 Pain Willingness Scale 20.21 (11.08) 28.08 (10.80) -8.92 P = 0.001 -9.60 -6.14 Roland Morris Disability Questionnaire Catastrophising Scale t-test of Level of 95% significance significance confidence interval Lower - Upper Expected t is 1.96 at a significant level of 0.05 249 Significant differences in mean scores between the samples were found on all scales except for the Roland and Morris Disability Questionnaire and the Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire total score. The Brazilian sample had higher scores on depression, self-efficacy and activity engagement than the Australian sample, and lower scores on catastrophising and pain willingness. Mean scores on the disability measure (RMDQ) indicate that these samples were similar on levels of self-reported disability. A similar trend occurred with levels of acceptance. What might be more important than their respective differences on these dimensions is whether or not different outcomes might be found (e.g. different roles of psychological factors in relation to disability). It is plausible to expect that as living conditions in a developing country such as Brazil may be more demanding (e.g. socio-economic disparities, lack of work, and competition) than in Australia. In Brazil, if an adult is unable to work, there is no financial support from the Government. Whereas in Australia, there is a universal welfare system that provides some financial support for the unemployed. Accordingly, Brazilians might be expected to be more resilient than Australians with chronic pain. This might be reflected in higher pain self-efficacy levels in the Brazilian chronic pain patients. The fact that the Brazilian sample had higher scores on depression and lower scores on catastrophising when compared to the Australian sample also raises some questions. Firstly, it confirms that although these two constructs are correlated they are distinct. Second, although 250 prevalence of depression varies in different countries and cultures (Bijl et al. 2003; Jorge 2003) in Latin cultures display of affect and suffering is more acceptable, thus Brazilians could be culturally encouraged to share their feelings Furthermore, and some cognitive studies symptoms have shown more that than Australians. depression can be associated with lower socio-economic level (Jorge 2003). It is also possible that key drivers of depression in the two countries might be different. This same rationale could be extended to catastrophising, but as noted, Australians had higher scores on catastrophising. Another explanation for this feature could be that in general Brazilians are usually thought to be very optimistic, have a strong faith in God and to have positive expectations about the future. Indeed, the researcher observed that when answering item 5 of the catastrophising scale (i.e. “I am a hopeless case”) several subjects verbally replied or exclaimed “no there is hope for me, I cannot say that”. This may illustrate that this type of belief is not socially acceptable. As outlined by several authors (e.g. Skevington, 1995; Raspe et al., 2004; Bates et al., 1993; Edwards et al., 2005), there is no a priori reason to expect noxious stimuli perception differences based in the biological basis, however there is evidence supporting intercultural differences in perceiving and reporting pain, as well as other psychological features. Therefore, the differences on scores of selfefficacy, catastrophising, depression and activity engagement and pain willingness are expected to occur due to inter-cultural differences. 251 In relation to catastrophising, although in the Brazilian culture displays of emotions are encouraged, not having a faith in future or being negative is discouraged. This may occur due to religious beliefs, since the Brazilian culture and social beliefs are strongly influenced by religion. As described by Rippentropa et al. (2005), religion/spirituality can have costs and benefits for the health of those with chronic pain. Therefore, it can be hypothesised that this positive outcome/feature (i.e. having lower levels of reported catastrophising than the Australian sample) may be related to this issue. Results from the Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire were mixed. Whilst there were no differences between the samples in the total mean score, Brazilians had higher mean scores in the activity engagement scale (which reflect engaging in normal life activities despite pain), while Australians had significant higher mean scores in the pain willingness scale (which reflects need to engage into activities to avoid pain). Due to the mixed results, lack of evidence and the novelty of this concept, it is unclear what to make of these differences, and further investigation of this issue is clearly needed. There was no significant difference between samples regarding gender on mean scores for all measures. As noted previously, disability, depression and catastrophising are reported to be moderated by gender differences (e.g. Sullivan et al., 2005), however these findings were not replicated in this study in both samples. There was no significant difference on mean scores on most of the measures in both samples regarding pain site. However, in the Brazilian 252 sample patients with pain in two or more sites scored significantly higher on disability and depression compared to the Australian sample. The findings that pain site contributed to disability is often reported in the literature, although with some variance (Von Korff et al. 1992). In general, multiple pain sites appear to be more disabling, although specific sites do not seem to play an important role on emotional adjustment (Von Korff et al. 1992). This finding was partially confirmed in these samples, since in the Brazilian sample, pain in more than one site was associated with higher levels of disability and depression, and predicted disability. Whilst in the Australian sample having pain in two or more sites was a predictor of unemployment. Regarding differences associated with educational level. In both samples participants with lower levels of education had higher scores on disability (p ≤ 0.01), which suggests that they are more disabled than those with higher levels of education. Although economic/social levels were not assessed in these series of studies, it can be expected that lower levels of education will be associated with lower socio-economic levels. The findings that lower levels of education (i.e. less than 10 years) were associated with higher scores on disability (measured by the RMDQ) is consistent with others in the literature in different cultures and countries (Blyth et al. 2001; Eriksen et al. 2003; Hagen et al. 2006; Tan et al. 1997; Vowles et al. 2004). 253 The relationship between scores on the different dimensions was also analysed. In both samples participants who were not working due to pain had lower scores on self-efficacy and higher scores on disability, depression and catastrophising than the participants who had pain but were still working. In the Brazilian sample the not working group also differed significantly on levels of acceptance. This did not occur in the Australian sample. This pattern suggests that in both samples, people that have chronic pain and are working are better adjusted emotionally and less physically disabled. Although the relationship between chronic pain, disability and psychological factors can be bi-directional, a number of studies have shown that some psychological factors can play an important role in transition from acute to chronic pain and in disability (Linton 2000; Linton 2005; Pincus et al. 2002). The relationship between socio-demographic, clinical and psychological variables and disability, depression and pain intensity also occurred in a slightly different fashion in these samples, but a similar trend was evident in both. 5.8.1 Variables contributing to disability in the Australian and in the Brazilian sample Only in the Australian sample did pain intensity contribute significantly to disability, while in the Brazilian sample level of education and pain site had a similar contribution to disability. Although different 254 factors contributed to disability in the Australian and in the Brazilian sample, as noted before similar findings have been reported in the literature (i.e. pain intensity) (Marhold er al., 2002). The contribution of educational level to pain prevalence and disability is often reported in the literature (e.g. Blyth et al., 2001), supporting the findings of Study Two as well. Due to socio-economic conditions, in the Brazilian sample it could be expected that people with lower levels of education are in jobs that are more physical in nature. As a result, this group could be subjected to higher physical loads and demands (when compared with those with higher educational levels), which could contribute to chronic pain and associated disability. The fact that level of education only contributed to disability in the Brazilian sample may reflect the contribution of social factors to chronic pain and disability, and the need to consider population, socio-economic and cultural specificities when treatments produced in developed countries or in other contexts are applied to the Brazilian population. For example, due to social contingencies level of disability in Brazil may be more influenced by biological and social factors than individual factors (i.e. psychological), while in more developed countries the availability of treatment for organic pathologies and more favourable socio-economic features may make individual factors (e.g. activity engagement) more important. As outlined earlier, it is not expected that the two samples would differ markedly in organic factors (i.e. type of pathology), therefore it can be hypothesised that socio-economic and cultural factors 255 have a different contribution to disability in the Brazilian sample than in the Australian. This issue will be further explored when comparing both samples on factors involved in prediction of work status. Among psychological variables, in both samples self-efficacy had the highest contribution to disability among all the psychological variables. However, in the Australian sample activity engagement contributed to disability as well, which did not occur in the Brazilian sample. These findings confirm the role of self-efficacy on disability (Altmaier et al. 1993; Arnstein 2000; Arnstein et al. 1999; Nicholas 2007; Nicholas and Asghari 2006), specially when considering that in the Brazilian sample pain intensity did not contribute to disability and that in the Australian sample self-efficacy had a higher contribution than pain intensity. The finding that self-efficacy was the most important predictor in both samples confirms the role of self-efficacy in disability as proposed by Bandura (1977) and Nicholas (2007). Based on these authors, the belief that one can cope with chronic pain or engage in activities despite pain makes a large contribution to levels of physical disability. The finding that pain willingness also contributed to disability, but only in the Australian sample also confirms McCracken and Eccleston findings (2005, 2006) to some extent. On the other hand, the lack of contribution of CPAQ and the activity engagement scale in both samples raises doubts about the CPAQ and the role of acceptance in relation to disability. 256 5.8.2 Variables contributing to depression in the Australian and in the Brazilian sample Regarding the contribution of socio-demographic and clinical variables to depression, in both samples these variables did not predict depression at significant levels. However in the Australian sample catastrophising, pain willingness and self-efficacy contributed to depression. Whilst in the Brazilian sample, catastrophising was the only predictor of depression. These findings confirm the role of psychological factors in depression associated with chronic pain. In both samples clinical factors (e.g. pain intensity and physical disability) were less important than the psychological factors. This confirms previous findings (e.g. Linton, 2000). The contribution of catastrophising to depression, which occurred in both samples, is often reported in the literature (eg. Sullivan et al., 2005; Nicholas and Asghari, 2006). However, findings reporting the contributions of self-efficacy and pain willingness to depression have also been reported (i.e. Nicholas and Asghari, 2006; McCracken and Eccleston, 2006 respectively). Therefore, while the role of catastrophising in relation to depression has been confirmed, the findings on the Australian sample suggest that other factors may contribute as well. This last finding could suggest that in different populations different factors might play a differing role in a specific outcome, such as depression. 257 5.8.3 Variables contributing to pain intensity in the Australian and in the Brazilian sample When examining the predictive factors for pain intensity, gender contributed to pain intensity in the Brazilian sample, but not in the Australian. Gender differences (female) have been found to be associated with higher reports of pain intensity (Keogh et al. 2006; Woodrow et al. 1972). Although in many societies it may be more acceptable for women to report pain and suffering than men (Hobara 2005), these findings have been replicated only in the Brazilian sample. On the other hand, the higher percentage of female subjects in both samples confirms the evidence described in the literature (i.e. there is different pain prevalence associated to gender and different responses to pain) (Blyth et al., 2001). Among psychological variables self-efficacy was the only predictor of pain intensity and occurred in a similar trend in both samples. Although, the contribution of self-efficacy to pain intensity has not been widely investigated, other findings that report the contribution of catastrophising, control and acceptance to pain intensity (e.g. Sullivan et al., 2005 and McCracken 2005 respectively) could support the role of self-efficacy in pain intensity. That is, a person’s belief in their ability to function despite pain (Nicholas 2007) may contribute to lessen the focus on pain, as well as provide some sense of control of the pain. This would have an effect opposite to what happens in people who catastrophise (i.e. keep or increase attention on pain or noxious stimuli). 258 5.8.4 Examining risk factors for work status in the Australian and in the Brazilian sample When examining risk factors for unemployment in both groups a similar pattern was found, but there were some different trends. Age and educational levels were significant risk factors for both samples, but educational level was the most important risk factor for unemployment and had a much higher odds ratio in the Brazilian sample. On the other hand, in the Australian sample pain site and depression were also risk factors for unemployment. While in the Brazilian sample physical disability and self-efficacy were risk factors for unemployment. Age and educational level are in general risk factors for unemployment independently of having chronic pain. For chronic pain patients, age and lower level of education or qualification have been found to be a barrier to return to work (Tan et al. 1997; Vowles et al. 2004; Watson et al. 2004), and can be considered an additional risk for unemployment in chronic pain patients in different countries. Nevertheless, the contribution of educational level to unemployment in the Brazilian sample (3.49 at a p=0.001), nearly 2 times more than in the Australian sample (1.94 at a p=0.03) is meaningful when considering that the rate of unemployment in Brazil (9.1%) is nearly two times higher than in Australia (4.9%) and that educational level might mediate unemployment in a developing country more than in a developed country (Lampreia 1995). Therefore, due to social contingencies (e.g. availability of jobs for disabled people), having 259 a chronic disease for people that have lower educational levels may increase the likelihood for unemployment in Brazil more than in Australia. Another major difference between the samples was that in the Australian sample among all variables depression was the most potent risk factor for unemployment, while in the Brazilian sample, depression did not attain a significant level. On the other hand, among the psychological variables self-efficacy obtained the higher odds ratio for the Brazilian sample, but did not reach significant levels in the Australian sample. Considering the characteristics of a developing country, the risk factors for unemployment found in the Brazilian sample are relevant and meaningful. Beside educational level and age, being physically disabled increases the risk for unemployment. Having a low self-efficacy belief in the ability to function despite pain, could also contribute to failure to return to work (or to remain at work). While, pain unemployment, site, being age and depressed education seems to are risk contribute factors largely for to unemployment in the Australian sample. The contribution of depression to unemployment has been reported in other studies conducted in developed countries (Marhold et al. 2002; Sullivan et al. 1992; Sullivan et al. 1998). It could be hypothesised that, considering the socio-economic characteristics of Australia (i.e. low level of unemployment and flexibility of working conditions) individual factors such as being depressed could be a major risk factor for not returning to work. 260 5.8.5 Summary of discussion Taken together, these findings suggest that in both samples biological, social and psychological factors interact dynamically and contribute to disability, depression, pain intensity and work status. In general, the Australian and the Brazilian sample presented a several similarities and only a few differences regarding the contribution of psychosocial factors to disability and emotional adjustment. These findings confirm the hypothesis that the relationship between self-efficacy beliefs, acceptance, catastrophising, depression and disability would occur in a similar pattern in these two samples and in accordance with broader biopsychosocial perspectives. Furthermore these findings are in accordance with the literature regarding ethnic variations in responses to pain (e.g. Bates et al., 1993; Edwards et al., 2001; Raspe et al., 2004; Skevington, 1995), specially when considering the findings regarding risk factors for unemployment. The bulk of evidence and perspectives suggest that biological factors cannot account for differences in perception of noxious stimuli, but cultures vary in the way they respond to chronic pain. On the other hand, there is evidence that ethnic differences regarding responses to pain seem to be smaller when ethnic groups are matched on confounding variables (i.e. socio-economic variables) (Tait and Chibnall 2005). This conclusion would suggest that the evaluation of patients with chronic pain and treatment for chronic pain based on models generated in other countries could be applied to the Brazilian population with chronic pain. 261 CHAPTER SIX General Discussion 6.1 General discussion In this section the main findings of this series of studies will be highlighted according to the existing literature in this field. The strengths and limitations of this study will be also presented, as well as suggestion for further research. Study One The findings of Study One suggest that the Roland and Morris Disability Questionnaire (Roland and Morris 1983), the Pain Self-Efficacy Questionnaire (Nicholas 1989), the DASS – Depression Scale (Lovibond and Lovibond 1995) and the PRSS – Catastrophising Scale (Flor et al. 1993) have adequate validity and reliability properties when used in a Brazilian chronic pain population. On the other hand the psychometric properties of the Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire (McCracken 1999) were not confirmed, except for the activity engagement scale. The results of the present study support previous findings (Flor et al. 1993; Lovibond and Lovibond 1995; Nicholas 1989; Nicholas 2007; Roland and Fairbank 2000; Roland and Morris 1983; Taylor et al. 2005) suggesting that all the studied measures are valid, reliable and that norms can be established to be used in the Brazilian chronic pain population. 262 Furthermore, when analysing the predictive properties of the above measures, correlational and regression analyses conducted in Study Two and Three confirmed the findings of Study One. This series of studies also examined the predictive properties of a number of socio-demographic and psychological factors on disability and depression associated with chronic pain, and tested whether the relationships among self-efficacy beliefs, acceptance, catastrophising, depression and disability in a Brazilian chronic pain sample were consistent with the evidence reported in the international pain literature and consistent with current biopsychosocial models of chronic pain. Study Two The findings of Study Two suggest that in the Brazilian sample although socio-demographic (i.e. educational level) and clinical variables (i.e. pain site) contribute to disability, self-efficacy had a greater contribution to disability as well as to pain intensity. The role of selfefficacy on disability has been reported elsewhere (Altmaier et al. 1993; Arnstein 2000; Arnstein et al. 1999; Keefe et al. 2004; Nicholas 2007; Nicholas and Asghari 2006; Turk and Okifuji 2002) and is supported by the findings of this study in a different culture than the ones investigated by most studies. These findings suggest that even when a number of socio-demographic, clinical and psychological variables are taken into account, self-efficacy seems to be an important predictor of disability. Indeed, in this Brazilian chronic pain sample self-efficacy was also an important risk factor for unemployment as well. 263 These findings reinforce Bandura’s (1977) position that selfefficacy is an important predictor of behaviour (e.g. reported disability, pain intensity and work status). The contribution of self-efficacy to disability is also in accordance with coping strategies models (Lazarus and Folkman, 1984 and Lazarus, 1993) which consider that coping strategies are also a function of beliefs, among other factors. Based on this perspective, it can be expected that chronic pain patients with low self-efficacy are likely to have ineffective coping strategies that may mediate physical disability (Turk 1999), as well as efforts to work despite pain. Indeed, recent evidence suggest that self-efficacy is one of the most important contributor to disability and a number of treatment outcomes (Keefe et al. 2004; Turner et al. 2007). When the contribution of socio-demographic, clinical and cognitive variables to depression was examined, catastrophising was the most important (and the only) predictor of depression. This finding is often reported in the literature, which suggests that catastrophising is a superior predictor of depression compared to other beliefs and coping responses (Sullivan and D'Eon 1990; Turner et al. 2000), acceptance (Nicholas and Asghari 2006; Viane et al. 2003), and self-efficacy and fear-avoidance (Nicholas and Asghari 2006). Altogether, these findings may suggest that the degree to which a chronic pain patient experiences depression is largely influenced by factors other than pain intensity, physical disability, coping and selfefficacy. As suggested by cognitive and behavioural models (Beck 1976; Sullivan and D'Eon 1990), cognitions (specifically catastrophising) 264 strongly mediate how people interpret the nature of their reality and consequently could possibly affect their mood. Thus, being depressed in the context of having chronic pain has to do not only with being in pain or disabled, but more to do with cognitive-evaluative processes. Regarding depression, the Geisser catastrophising relationship et contribute al. between (1999) more to pointed the catastrophising out evaluative that and it and seems affective dimensions of pain. Therefore, based in this conception it could be expected that catastrophising would have an important contribution to depression; especially if depression is understood as one of the consequences of pain. This view is also is accordance with the existence of three dimensions of catastrophising (i.e. rumination, magnification and helplessness) as suggested by Sullivan et al. (1995) or two dimensions (i.e. rumination and helplessness) as found in Study One and supported by other studies (Chibnall and Tait 2005). As it can be observed these dimensions are related to evaluative and affective components of pain, and therefore it could be expected that they would interact more with other dimensions from the same sphere. The finding that in Study 2 factor 2 of the catastrophising scale (i.e. helplessness) was a stronger predictor of depression than factor 1 (i.e. rumination) confirms the evidence that catastrophic beliefs have cognitive and affective dimensions (Sullivan et al. 1995). The finding that the helplessness component of catastrophising would have a more important contribution to depression than the other 265 dimension found (i.e. rumination) is in accordance with the pain literature on the relationship between depression and catastrophising (Keefe et al. 2004; Sullivan et al. 2001b). The finding that acceptance did not contribute significantly to disability, depression and pain intensity was at variance with some previous findings reported in the literature (i.e. McCracken et al., 2003; 2004; 2005; 2006). These differences may be due to measurement problems with the CPAQ and the fact that McCracken et al. (1999; 2004) did not control for the possible effects of other cognitive variables such as catastrophising, fear-avoidance and self-efficacy. Clearly, further research should be conducted on this concept in relation to chronic pain (e.g. Nicholas and Asghari, 2006). Depression also did not contribute to disability and pain intensity in the studied sample. Although there is a high prevalence of depression in chronic pain patients and the role of depression in chronic pain is recognised (i.e. poor responses to treatment), the nature of this relationship remains unclear (Worz, 2003). Broadly there are three models that describe the relationship of chronic pain and depression (i.e. antecedent, consequence and bidirectional hypotheses (Worz 2003). Most of the evidence suggests that depression may follow chronic pain (Fishbain et al., 1997). In addition, the relationship between pain intensity, disability and depression has been widely reported in the literature, but with a large variance in findings (McCracken and Eccleston 2005; Turk et al. 1995), These variance on results may occur due to several reasons; one of them 266 may be related to measurement problems (Pincus and Williams 1999; Taylor et al. 2005). Since a number of measures used in these studies have somatic items, this feature may inflate scores and thus increase the contribution of depression to a number of outcomes. Furthermore, some studies have reported that depression does not appear to be an important predictor of physical disability, especially when compared to other factors such as catastrophising and self-efficacy (e.g. Nicholas and Asghari, 2006; Turner et al., 2007). Therefore, when taking into account only cognitive symptoms of depression, it may be possible that depression will lose its power to predict disability, and may be better understood as a consequence of having chronic pain, which may mediate other outcomes. Altogether, the findings of this study are in accordance with the literature, and although depression did not contribute to disability and pain intensity in this population, it seems reasonable to expect that depression will somehow influence other pain-related outcomes indirectly (e.g. number of consultations, use of medication). When examining risk factors for unemployment in the Brazilian sample, educational level, age, physical disability and self-efficacy were found to be significant predictors of unemployment. However it is important to mention that in this series of studies, in the chronic pain context unemployment was interpreted as an outcome or an indicator of disability, and a number of variables that are related to work status (i.e. work conditions) were not analysed. 267 The finding that age, educational level and disability were risk factors for unemployment is in accordance with findings reported in studies conducted in developed countries (e.g. Vowles et al., 2004). These findings are clinically meaningful and in accordance with biopsychosocial perspective of chronic pain. Furthermore, in general, age and educational level can be considered risk factors for unemployment even for healthy subjects, so when considering that these aspects were investigated in patients with chronic pain in a developing country it would be expected that they would occur and contribute to unemployment even in a higher magnitude. The contribution of self-efficacy to unemployment in patients with chronic pain has not received much attention, but there is some evidence that self-efficacy is related to work status in people with chronic pain (Adams and Williams 2003; Cohen et al. 2000). The present study’s findings that self-efficacy was predictive of work status is therefore consistent with these earlier findings and suggests the topic merits further investigation. While other work related factors (e.g. social security, availability of modified work duties) may contribute to work status (Teasell and Bombardier 2001), self-efficacy beliefs, as an individual factor, seems to make an important contribution to work status; in a similar fashion that it occurred to physical disability. As outlined by a number of authors (e.g. Adams and Williams, 2003), the work status of chronic pain patients seems to depend less on 268 medical variables than psychosocial factors. The findings of this study although with some limitations are in accordance with this evidence. Study Three In Study Three, the same variables examined in Study Two were tested in an Australian sample with chronic pain and compared with the findings of the Brazilian sample and the current literature. In the Australian sample pain intensity, self-efficacy and pain willingness contributed significantly to disability. Regarding the contribution of socio-demographic, clinical and psychological factors to depression, in the Australian sample catastrophising, pain willingness and self-efficacy contributed to depression. In relation to the contribution of socio-demographic, clinical and psychological factors to pain intensity, self-efficacy was the only variable to attain a significant p level. Regarding work status, pain site, educational level, age and depression were significant risk factors for unemployment in the Australian sample. Most of the findings of the Australian sample confirmed the results that occurred in the Brazilian sample and have been discussed before. That is: self-efficacy was the strongest predictor of disability and pain intensity, and catastrophising was an important predictor of depression. However, in the Australian sample other factors contributed to depression as well (i.e. pain willingness and self-efficacy, in order of magnitude). The contribution of self-efficacy and pain willingness to depression has not been often examined, but it has been reported previously by Nicholas and Asghari (2006) and McCracken et al. (2004), respectively. 269 In the Australian sample it could be hypothesised that high scores on self-efficacy (i.e. having the belief in one’s ability to engage in normal activities despite pain) would contribute to less emotional suffering and depression. As noted before, broadly speaking the cognitive and behavioural model (Beck 1976; Sullivan and D'Eon 1990) outline that cognitions (e.g. catastrophising) mediate how people interpret the nature of their reality. Accordingly, being confident in one’s ability to deal with chronic pain and disability would minimize feelings of helplessness and promote a sense of control regarding this situation. The same rationale could be extended to pain willingness (i.e recognition that avoidance and control often do not work to adapt to chronic pain), that is; patients that would have an accepting attitude towards pain would not engage in efforts to avoid or control pain, or would be prepared to engage in activities while in pain. It could be hypothesised that this attitude would generate less frustration, suffering and feelings or depressive thoughts. The concepts of self-efficacy and acceptance (measured by the activity engagement and pain willingness scales) have some similar features and both have been found to make a contribution to depression (Nicholas and Asghari, 2006; McCracken and Eccleston, 2005). This raises the question of whether they are different concepts. Self-efficacy and acceptance involve acknowledging pain, however self-efficacy also involves the recognition of personal abilities to cope with pain or carry on with activities while in pain, whilst acceptance involves only an attitude towards pain and not efforts to deal with pain. Thus, 270 close inspection does reveal conceptual differences, as outlined by McCracken et al., (2004a). Even so, it could be hypothesised that both factors would lead to less stress while experiencing pain. It is generally agreed that pain is a multidimensional phenomenon and that people living with chronic pain vary substantially in their responses to it (Morley and Keefe 2007). Although a number of factors have been adjustment identified (i.e. as important acceptance, contributors catastrophising, to disability self-efficacy, and fear- avoidance) (Keefe et al. 2004), it is likely that they may share some variance, and that for some people, populations and in different cultures these factors may play slightly different roles. For example, self-efficacy may have a more important contribution to disability than acceptance for patients who are trying to return to work than those that are making an injury related insurance claim. While acceptance might be more important to disability than self-efficacy in patients who have chronic pain due to cancer and are in a terminal stage. This is only speculation but it illustrates the possibility that the contribution of different variables to disability and adjustment may vary according to circumstances. As outlined by Vlaeyen and Morley (2005), it can be expected that different people and populations may benefit from interventions aimed at different factors involved in disability and adjustment. When comparing the contribution of psychosocial factors to work status between the Australian and Brazilian sample, the findings confirmed the role of educational level and age as risk factors for unemployment in 271 both samples. As noted, these findings have been reported in the pain literature as important risk factors for unemployment (Hagen et al. 2006; Vowles et al. 2004). It is also important to notice that level of unemployment in both samples was similar (41%), suggesting that despite having different sociodemographic and economic conditions in both countries chronic pain has a large impact on patients living with chronic pain. Nevertheless, the contribution of educational level to work status in the Brazilian sample was nearly twice as high as in the Australian sample. When taking into account the socio-economic conditions of both countries this finding may reflect the impact of social factors on pain outcomes (i.e. work status). As discussed previously, level of education is not always a predictor of work status but is frequently a predictor of higher income. The higher contribution of educational level to work status found in the Brazilian sample, suggests that in a developing country having a chronic illness (i.e. chronic pain) and lower educational level may increase the risk for unemployment. This may also occur in a developed country, but seemingly to a lesser extent. The contribution of depression as a risk factor for unemployment that occurred in the Australian sample has been reported elsewhere (e.g. Marhold er al., 2002) in similar conditions (i.e. developed countries), but did not occur in the Brazilian sample. Therefore, it could be hypothesised that in Australia depression might be more influential to work status than 272 in Brazil, where the contingencies mean you have to keep working regardless of how you feel. Since there is a greater availability and flexibility of jobs and a welfare system in developed countries, being at work may be more influenced by affective factors (i.e. depression); understood here as motivational factors rather than cognitions (i.e. self-efficacy). This picture seems not to be the case in Brazil where being disabled (which had the second highest odds ratio among all significant variables) and having a belief in the ability to perform an activity despite pain were stronger determinants of work status. In general, the findings of this series of studies confirmed the biopsychosocial perspective of chronic pain, since they showed that biological factors interact with social-demographic and psychological factors in shaping disability and emotional adjustment in people with chronic pain in a similar pattern, but in a slightly different way in two different social contexts. The slight differences that occurred in these two samples may be due to sample differences, but also due to economic and cultural factors. Hence, in different populations different factors might play a differing role in a specific outcome, such as depression or unemployment due to pain. As proposed by some ethnic/social models of pain (e.g. Skevington, 1995; Bates, 1987) social learning is instrumental in the development of meanings and attitudes toward pain. Ethnic/social perspectives regarding chronic pain poses that attitudes, beliefs, attribution, expectations and assumptions are shaped 273 socially and that culture has an important role in pain perception and response to pain and thus should be taken into account. However, it also seems that when ethnic groups are matched on confounding variables (i.e. income or social classes), these differences are relatively small (Edwards et al. 2005; Portenoy et al. 2004). Although, the Australian and the Brazilian sample were matched for gender and age, and did not differ significantly on a number of other variables such as pain intensity, pain site, pain duration, social class and other confounding variables (e.g. work related factors) were not controlled. Therefore other unknown variables could interfere in the studied outcome. The differences between samples regarding risk factors for work status (which were the biggest differences between samples) would be expected to occur as work status can be considered a variable that is more likely to be influenced by social contingencies (i.e. culture and demographic-economic factors) than physical disability, depression and pain intensity. Nevertheless, if other variables were studied (i.e. pain behaviour, factors involved in compensation) other differences might appear due to the fact that they are also mediated by other social contingencies. However in general, the findings in both samples occurred in a similar trend, which could be expected as both countries share a Western cultural orientation, when compared to other cultures such as countries in the Middle East or Asia. 274 Furthermore, data was collected in South and Southeast of Brazil, which has a large number of European immigrants and it is more influenced by western cultures when compared to other regions of Brazil. It can be hypothesized that although there are differences regarding responses to pain among different groups; that should be taken into account. It seems that pain is a universal phenomenon, and chronic pain patients may tend to share more similarities than differences. 6.2 Limitations and strengths Limitations Although the limitations of Study One have been previously described, in this section the limitations of this series of studies will be acknowledged in a broader way. The limitations of this series of studies include collection of data using only psychometric questionnaires, the cross-sectional design, and to what extent the results could be generalised to the Brazilian population with chronic pain. All data were obtained by self-report measures, which could lead to a number of problems: shared variance could contribute to the magnitude of some correlations; lack of reliability and/or validity of the measures; and factors influencing test answer (i.e. recall bias). Furthermore, when examining the reliability of the measures, test re-test reliabilities was not conducted due to the difficulties in contacting 275 subjects for a second application of the questionnaires; however other reliability analyses were conducted. Nevertheless, when considering the subjective nature of the topic collecting data without using questionnaires would be difficult to do. Furthermore, one of the main goals of this series of studies was to validate a number of relevant psychometric measures to be used in the Brazilian chronic pain population. On the other hand, self-report is considered being the most direct way to access pain dimensions (Turk and Melzack, 2001). Observing a few statistical parameters and guaranteeing the psychometric properties of the measures would minimize the influence of these problematic issues (Jensen 2003). The cross-sectional design of these studies also imposes limits to any consideration of causal relationships between the studied variables and their directions. Thus, findings based on cross-sectional data should be supported with further replication and prospective studies. Another limitation of this research concerns the sample employed. The sample was not selected to be representative of Brazilian or Australian chronic pain patients and these impose some barriers to the generalizability of these findings. In fact, the sample was a convenience sample constituted of chronic pain patients attending certain pain clinics. This may produce some biased information, since this sample may consist of subjects that did not respond to pain as successfully as a nonclinical pain population. It is also important to acknowledge that some of the differences found between the present study and other studies may 276 be due to differences in samples, cultural differences and health system contingencies. Therefore, the findings of this series of studies cannot be generalised to the Brazilian and Australian chronic pain population, but the degree to which the findings were consistent with the literature does provide some support for their generalizability. Strengths On the other hand, these studies have a number of strengths. The psychometric properties of the questionnaires were examined and proven to be reliable and valid, which enhance the quality of the data collected. Second, although not representative, the sample size is moderate (311 in each country), heterogeneous and comes from nine chronic pain facilities in two regions of Brazil and the largest chronic pain centre in Australia, which permits generalisation of the findings to some extent. This study is also one of the first studies to analyse the simultaneous contribution of a number of socio-demographic and clinical variables and self-efficacy, acceptance and catastrophising to disability, work status, pain intensity and emotional adjustment. Considering these limitations and strengths, this series of studies provides support for the use of a number of pain measures with Brazilians with chronic pain (increasing considerably the number of available psychological measures from 16 to 21). This series of studies have also supported the existing evidence about the role of cognition in adjustment to chronic pain, and they confirmed some of the features of 277 biopsychosocial models of chronic pain and the validity of internationally produced evidence in this Brazilian population. Furthermore, since most findings of this series of studies were in accordance with the literature confirming biopsychosocial models of pain, to an extent it could also be expected that treatment for chronic pain patients based on them could be applicable to Brazilians with chronic pain. 6.3 Conclusion This series of studies aimed to explore some key cognitive and affective aspects of pain in a Brazilian and an Australian sample based on the premises of biopsychosocial models of chronic pain. The major findings of this series of studies are in accordance with the literature that suggest that a number of psychosocial factors may shape the individual’s pain experience, influencing the degree to which pain is experienced, responses to it, and the degree of interference caused by pain (Keefe et al. 2004; Linton 2000; Pincus et al. 2002; Skevington 1995; Turk and Okifuji 2002; Turner et al. 2007) The major findings of this series of studies have partially confirmed the hypotheses on which this research project was based on, that is: 1. Pain self-efficacy beliefs, acceptance, catastrophising and depression are significant predictors of disability; 2. Pain self-efficacy beliefs, acceptance, catastrophising and disability are significant predictors of depression; 278 3. Socio-demographic and psychological variables are risk factors for unemployment due to chronic pain; 4. The relationship between cognitions, depression and disability occur in a similar pattern in the Australian and in the Brazilian samples. Although these hypotheses were stated in a general fashion, it was not expect that all the psychological variables would equally contribute or predict disability, work status, depression and pain intensity. As matter of fact, based on the literature reviewed it was expected that self-efficacy would be an important predictor of disability and that catastrophising would contribute to depression and pain intensity. Educational level, age and some clinical variables were expected to be risk factors for unemployment. It was also expected that similar findings would occur in both samples but with some variation. Furthermore the biopsychosocial perspective of chronic pain was expected to be confirmed in the Brazilian chronic pain population. A number of findings from these studies are in accordance with the international literature and the biopsychosocial perspective; which confirms that cognitions play an important role in disability and emotional adjustment even in different populations. From a clinical perspective, since these findings have confirmed the current biopsychosocial model of chronic pain, clinical treatment procedures (based on this model) developed in other countries may be used effectively in the Brazilian population with chronic pain. Thus, the 279 use of multidisciplinary pain treatments, like CBT pain management programs (Blyth et al. 2005), would seem to be warranted in Brazil. Furthermore, due to the availability of internationally used measures provided by this series of studies, as well as the confirmation of the international literature, this series of studies may contribute to improve research in this area in Brazil and collaborative work with other countries. 6.4. Further directions It is important to replicate and expand these findings in other chronic pain population in Brazil and worldwide. Although this study was conducted in a large sample, it was not representative and probably there are cultural, across-person and within-person differences that should be explored. Due to the fact that the present study was cross-sectional, it is possible that the relationships between the examined variables may vary over time. Therefore, further prospective studies should investigate and determine process involved in the development of chronic pain and factors affecting this population. There is also room for improvement regarding theoretical and measurement issues involving acceptance and catastrophising (i.e. relevant dimensions of acceptance and catastrophising, validity and reliability of the Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire). The use of the validated measures should be encouraged in Brazilian clinical settings in treatment evaluation and in identifying 280 treatment targets. Further research could also investigate the effectiveness of multidimensional interventions for this population. Considering that a number of cognitions and depression were considered simultaneously when examining their contribution to disability and emotional adjustment, and the fact that self-efficacy played an important role in disability, the contribution of self-efficacy should be investigated further. 281 References Adams, J.H. and Williams, A.C.d.C., What affects return to work for graduates of a pain management program with chronic upper limb pain, J. Occ Rehab, 13 (2003) 91-106. Aggarwal, V.R., Macfarlane, T.V. and Macfarlane, G.J., Why is pain more common amongst people living in areas of low socio-economic status? A populationbased cross-sectional study., British Dental Journal, 194 (2003) 383-87. Altmaier, E.M., Russel, D.W., Kao, C.F., Lehmann, T.R. and Weinstein, J.N., Role of self-efficacy in rehabilitation outcome among chronic low back pain patients, Journal of Counseling Psychology, 40 (1993) 335-39. American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, APA, Washington DC, 1994. Anastasi, A. and Urbina, S., Psychological Testing, Prentice-Hall, New York, 1997. Anderson, K.O., Dowds, B.N., Pelletz, R.E., Edwards, W.T. and Peeters-Asdourian, C., Development and initial validation of a scale to measure self-efficacy beliefs in patients with chronic pain, Pain, 63 (1995) 77-84. Andersson, G.B.J., Epidemiological features of chronic low-back pain, The Lancet, 354 (1999) 581-85. Arnstein, P., The mediation of disability by self-efficacy in different samples of chronic pain patients, Disability and Rehabilitation, 22 (2000) 794-801. Arnstein, P., Caudill, M., Mandle, C.L., Norris, A. and Beasley, R., Self efficacy as a mediator of the relationship between pain intensity, disability and depression in chronic pain patients, Pain, 80 (1999) 483-91. Asghari, A., The relative contributions of personality and pain-related beliefs/coping strategies in adjustment to chronic pain, School of Psychology, The University of New South Wales, NSW, 1996. Asghari, A. and Nicholas, M.K., Pain self-efficacy beliefs and pain behaviour. A prospective study, Pain, 94 (2001) 85-100. Australian Bureau of Statistics, Year Book Australia 2006. In: D. Trewin (Ed.), Vol. 2006, Australian Bureau of Statistics, Canberra, 2006. Australian Bureau of Statistics., Year Book Australia 2004. In: D. Trewin (Ed.), Year Book Australia, Australian Bureau of Statistics, Canberra, 2004. Aylard, P.R., Gooding, J.H., McKenna, P.J. and Snaith, R.P., A validation study of three anxiety and depression self-assessment scales, Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 31 (2005) 261-68. Bandura, A., Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioural change, Psychological Review, 84 (1977) 191-215. Bandura, A., O'Leary, A., Taylor, C.B., Gauthier, J. and Gossard, D., Perceived selfefficacy and pain control: Opioid and nonopioid mechanism., Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53 (1987) 406-14. Bandura, A., Taylor, C.B., Williams, S.L., Melford, I.N. and Barchas, J.D., Cathecolamine secretion as a function of perceived coping self-efficacy., Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53 (1985) 406-14. Banks, S.M. and Kerns, R.D., Explaining high rates of depression in chronic pain: A diathesis-stress framework, Psychological Bulletin, 119 (1996) 95-110. Bates, M.S., Ethnicity and Pain: A Biocultural Model, Soc. Sci. Med., 24 (1987) 4750. 282 Bates, M.S., Edwards, W.T. and Anderson, K.O., Ethnocultural influences on variation in chronic pain perception, Pain (1993) 101-12. Battié, M.C. and May, L., Physical and occupational therapy assessment approaches. In: D.C. Turk and R. Melzack (Eds.), Handbook of Pain Assessment, The Guilford Press, New York, 2001, pp. 204-19. Beck, A.T., Cognitive Therapy and the Emotional Disorders, International University Press, New York, 1976. Beck, A.T., Ward, C.H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J. and Erbaugh, J., An inventory to measure depression, Arch. Gen. Psychiat., 56 (1961) 53-63. Bejlland, I., Dahl, A.A., Haugh, T.T. and Neckelmann, D., The validity of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale An updated literature review, Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 52 (2002) 69-77. Bennett, D.H. and Holmes, D.S., Influence of denial (situational redefinition) and projection on anxiety associated with threat to self-esteem., Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32 (1975) 915-21. Berber, J.d.S.S., Kupek, E. and Berber, S.C., Prevalência de depressão e sua relação com a qualidade de vida em pacientes com síindrome da fibromialgia, Revista Brasileira de Reumatologia, 45 (2005) 47-54. Bergner, M., Bobbitt, R., Carter, W.B. and Gilson, B.S., The Sickness Impact Profile: Development and final revision of a health status measure, Medical Care, XIX (1981) 787-806. Bijl, R.V., de Graaf, R., Hiripi, E., Kessler, R.C., Kohn, R., Offord, D.R., Ustun, T.B., Vicente, B., Vollebergh, W.A., Walters, E.E. and Wittchen, H.U., The prevalence of treated and untreated mental disorders in five countries., Health Affairs, 22 (2003) 122-33. Blyth, F.M., March, L.M., Brnabic, A.J.M., Jorm, L.R., Williamson, M. and Cousins, M.J., Chronic pain in Australia: a prevalence study, Pain, 89 (2001) 127-34. Blyth, F.M., March, L.M., Nicholas, M.K. and Cousins, M.J., Self-management of chronic pain: a population-based study, Pain, 113 (2005) 285-92. Boothby, J.L., Thorn, B.E., Stroud, M.W. and Jensen, M.P., Coping with pain. In: R.J. Gatchel and D.C. Turk (Eds.), Psychosocial Factors in Pain: Critical Perspectives, Guilford Press., New York, 1999, pp. 343-59. Botega, N.J., Bio, M.R., Zomignani, M.A., Garcia, C. and Pereira, W.A., Transtornos do humor em enfermaria de clinica medica e validação de escala de medida (HAD) de ansiedade e depressão, Rev Saúde Pública, 29 (1995) 355–63. Bradley, L.A. and McKendree-Smith, N.L., Assessment of psychological status using interviews and self-report instruments. In: D.C. Turk and R. Melzack (Eds.), Handbook of Pain Assessment, Guilford Press, New York, 2001, pp. 292-319. Bradley, L.E., Cognitive-Behavioural Therapy for Chronic Pain. In: G.R. J. and D.C. Turk (Eds.), Psychological Approaches to Pain Management : A Practitioner's Handbook., Guilford Press, New York, 1996, pp. 131-47. Breivik, H., Collett, B., Ventafridda, V., Cohen, R. and Gallacher, D., Survey of chronic pain in Europe: Prevalence, impact on daily life and treatment, Journal of Pain, 10 (2006) 287-333. Brewin, C.R., Cognitive Foundations of Clinical Psychology, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Ltda., Exeter, 1988. 283 Bronfort, G. and Bouter, L.M., Responsiveness of general health status in chronic low back pain: a comparison of the COOP Charts and the SF-36, Pain, 83 (1999) 201-09. Brown, G.K. and Nicassio, P.M., The development of a questionnaire for the assessment of active and passive coping strategies in chronic pain patients., Pain, 31 (1987) 53-65. Brown, G.K., Wallston, K.A. and Nicassio, P.M., Social support and depression in rheumatoid arthritis., Journal of Applied Social Psychology., 19 (1989) 1164181. Burton, A.K., Tillotson, K.M., Main, C.J. and Hollis, S., Psychosocial predictors of outcome in acute and subacute low back trouble., Spine (1995) 722-28. Campbell, D.T. and Fiske, D., Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix, Psychological Bulletin, 56 (1959) 81-105. Campbell, L.C., Clauw, D.J. and Keefe, F.J., Persistent pain and depression: A biopsychosocial perspective, Society of Biological Psychiatry, 54 (2003) 399409. Cassou, B., Derriennic, F., Monfort, C., Norton, J. and Touranchet, A., Chronic neck and shoulder pain, age, working conditions: longitudinal results from a large random sample min France, Occup Environ Med., 59 (2002) 537-44. Catala, E., Reig, E., Artes, M., Aliaga, L., Lopez, J.S. and Segu, J.L., Prevalence of pain in the Spanish population: telephone survey in 5000 homes., European Journal of Pain, 6 (2002) 133-40. Chapman, W. and Jones, C., Variations in cutaneous and visceral pain sensitivity in normal subjects., Journal Clini Invest, 23 (1994) 81-91. Chibnall, J.T. and Tait, R.C., Confirmatory factor analysis of the pain Catastrophizing Scale in African American and Caucasian Workers' Compensation claimants with low back injuries, Pain, 113 (2005) 369-375. Ciconelli, R.M., Ferraz, M.B. and Santos, W., Tradução para a língua portuguesa e validação do questionário genérico de avaliação de qualidade de vida SF-36 (Brasil SF-36), Rev Bras Reumatologia, 39 (1998) 143-50. Cleeland, C.S., Nakamura, Y., Mendoza, T.R., Edwards, K.R., Douglas, J. and Serlim, R.C., Dimensions of the impact of cancer pain in a four country sample: new information from multidimensional scaling., Pain, 67 (1996) 267-73. Cohen, M., Nicholas, M.K. and Blanch, A., Medical assessment and management of work-related low back or neck/arm pain, Journal Occup Health Safety, 16 (2000) 307-17. Cunha, J.A., Manual da versão em Português das escalas Beck, Casa do Psicologo, Sao Paulo, 2001. da Silva, M.C., Fassa, A.G. and Valle, N.C.J., Dor lombar crônica em uma população adulta do Sul do Brasil: prevalência e fatores associados, Cadernos de Saúde Pública, 20 (2004) 15-22. Davidson, M. and Keating, J.L., A comparison of five low back disability questionnaires: reliability and responsiveness, Physical Therapy, 82 (2002) 824. DeGood, D.E., Relationship of pain-coping strategies to adjustment and functioning. In: R.J. Gatchel and J.N. Weisberg (Eds.), Personality Characteristics of 284 Patients with Pain, American Psychological Association, Washington, 2000, pp. 129-63. DeGood, D.E. and Tait, R.C., Assessment of pain beliefs and pain coping. In: D.C. Turk and R. Melzack (Eds.), Handbook of Pain Assessment, Guilford Press, New York, 2001, pp. 320-337. D'eon, J.L., Harris, C.A. and Ellis, J.A., Testing Factorial Validity and Gender Invariance of the Pain Catastrophizing Scale, Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 27 (2004) 361-72. Deyo, R.A., Comparative validity of the Sickness Impact Profile and shorter scales for functional assessment in low-back pain, Spine, 11 (1986) 951-54. Dimsdale, J.E., Stalked by the past: The influence of ethnicity on health, Psychosomatic Medicine, 62 (2000) 161-70. Dionne, C.E., Von Korff, M., Koepsell, T.D., Deyo, R.A., Barlow, W.I. and Checkoway, H., Formal education and back pain: A review, Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 55 (2001) 455-68. Donald, L.P., Deyo, R.A., Atlas, S.J., Singer, D.E., Chapin, A. and Keller, R.B., Assessing Health-Related Quality of Life in Patients With Sciatica, Spine, 20 (1995) 1899-1909. Eccleston, C., Role of psychology in pain management, Br J Anaesth, 87 (2001) 14452. Edwards, C.L., Fillingim, R.B. and Keefe, F., Race, ethnicity and pain., Pain, 94 (2001a) 133-37. Edwards, R.R., Doleys, D.M., Fillingim, R.B. and Lowery, D., Ethnic differences in pain tolerance: clinical implications in a chronic pain population., Psychosomatic Medicine, 63 (2001b) 316-23. Edwards, R.R. and Fillingim, R.B., Ethnic differences in thermal responses., Psychosomatic Medicine, 61 (1999) 346-54. Edwards, R.R., Moric, M., Hustfeldt, B., Buvanendran, A. and Ivankovich, O., Ethnic similarities and differences in the chronic pain experience: a comparison of African American, Hispanic and White patients, Pain Medicine, 6 (2005) 88-98. Elliott, G.R. and Eisdorfer, C., Stress and Human Health., Springer, New York, 1982. Ellis, A., Reason and Emotion in Psychotherapy, Lyle Stuart, New York, 1962. Engel, G.L., The need for a new medical model: A challenge for biomedicine, Science, 196 (1977) 129-36. Enthoven, P., Skargren, E., J., C. and Oberg, B., Predictive factors for 1-year and 5year outcome for disability in a working population of patients with low back pain treated in primary care, Pain, 122 (2006) 137-44. Eriksen, J., Jensen, M.K., Sjogren, P., Ekholm, O. and Rasmussen, N.K., Epidemiology of chronic non-malignant pain in Denmark, Pain, 106 (2003) 213-4. Evers, A.W.M., Kraaimaat, F.W., Van Lankveld, W., Jongen, P.J.H., Jacobs, J.W.G. and Bijlsma, J.W.J., Beyond unfavorable thinking: the Illness Cognition Questionnaire for chronic diseases, Journal Consult Clin Psychology, 69 (2001) 1026-1036. Fairbank, J.C.T., Davies, J.B., Couper, J. and O'Brien, J.P., The Oswestry Low-Back Pain Disability Questionnaire, Physiotherapy, 66 (1980) 271-73. 285 Fallo, T.B., Ethnic differences in attitudes to pain and preferred coping strategies, School of Psychology, Flinders University of South Australia, Flinders, 1998. Figueiró, J.A.B., Aspectos psicológicos e psiquiátricos da experiência dolorosa. In: M.M.M.J. de Carvalho (Ed.), Dor - Um estudo Multidisciplinar, Vol. I, Summus, São Paulo, 1999, pp. 140-58. Fishbain, D.A., Cutler, R., Rosomoff, H.L. and Rosomoff, R.S., Chronic painassociated depression: Antecedent or consequence of chronic pain? A Review., Clinical Journal of Pain, 13 (1997) 116-37. Fishbein, M. and Ajzen, I., Belief, Attitude, Intention and Behavior: An Introduction to Theory and Research, Addison-Wesley Publishing Company, Massachusetts., 1975. Flor, H., Behle, D.J. and Birbaumer, N., Assessment of Pain-Related Cognitions in Chronic Pain Patients, Behav. Res. Ther., 31 (1993) 63-73. Flor, H., Birbaumer, N., Schugens, M.M. and Lutzenberger, W., Symptom-specific psychophysiological responses in chronic pain patients., Psychophysiology, 29 (1992) 452-60. Flor, H., Birbaumer, N. and Turk, D.C., The psychobiology of chronic pain, Advances in behavioural research and therapy, 12 (1990) 47-84. Flor, H. and Hermann, C., Biopsychosocial models of pain. In: R.H. Dworkin and W.S. Breitbart (Eds.), Psychosocial Aspects of Pain: A Handbook for Health Care Providers, Vol. Progress in Pain Research and Management. Volume 27, IASP Press, Seattle, 2004, pp. 47-78. Flor, H. and Turk, D.C., Chronic back pain and rheumatoid arthritis: predicting pain and disability from cognitive variables., Journal of Behavioural Medicine, 11 (1988) 251-265. Flor, H., Turk, D.C. and Birbaumer, N., Assessment of stress-related psychophysiological responses in chronic pain patients., Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 35 (1985) 354-64. Gamsa, A., The role of psychological factors in chronic pain: I. A half century of study., Pain, 57 (1994) 5-16. Geiser, D.S., A comparison of acceptance-focused and control-focused psychological treatments in a chronic pain treatment center, Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Nevada, Reno, 1992. Geisser, M.E., Robinson, M.E. and Henson, C.D., The Coping Strategies Questionnaire and Chronic pain Adjustment: A Conceptual and Empirical Reanalysis, Clinical Journal of Pain, 10 (1994) 98-106. Geisser, M.E., Robinson, M.E., Miller, Q.L. and bade, S.M., Psychosocial factors and functional capacity evaluation among persons with chronic pain, Journal of Occupational Rehabilitation, 13 (2003) 259-76. Geisser, M.E., Robinson, M.E. and Riley III, J.L., Pain beliefs, coping and adjustment to chronic pain: Let's focus more on the negative., Pain Forum, 8 (1999) 161-68. Giardino, N.D., Jensen, M.P., Turner, J.A., Ehde, D.M. and Cardenas, D.D., Social environment moderates the association between catastrophizing and pain among persons with a spinal cord injury, Pain, 106 (2003) 19-25. Gibson, L. and Strong, J., The reliability and validity of a measure of perceived functional capacity for work in chronic pain, J. Occ Rehab, 6 (1996) 159-75. 286 Gottlieb, B.S., Development of the Pain Belief Questionnaire: A preliminary report. In: A.f.t.A.o.B. Therapy (Ed.), Philadelphia, 1984. Green, C.R., Baker, T.A., Sato, Y., Washington, T.L. and Smith, E.M., Race and chronic pain: A comparative study of young Black and White Americans presenting for management., The Journal of Pain, 4 (2003) 176-83. Grotle, M., Brox, J.I. and Vollestad, N.K., Cross-cultural adaptation of the Norwegian versions of the Roland-Morris Disability Questionnaire and the Oswestry Disability Index, Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine, 35 (2003) 241-47. Gueldof, E.L.M., Vinck, J., Vlaeyen, J.W.S., Hidding, A. and Cronbez, G., The differential role of pain, work characteristics and pain-related fear in explaining back pain and sick leave in occupational settings, Pain, 113 (2005) 71-81. Guillemin, F., Bombardier, C. and Beaton, D., Cross-cultural adaptation of healthrelated quality of life measures: Literature review and proposed guidelines, Journal Clinical Epidemiology, 46 (1993) 1417-432. Gureje, O., Von Korff, M., Simon, G.E. and Gater, R., Persistent pain and wellbeing: A World Health Organization study in primary care system., JAMA, 280 (1998) 147-51. Hadjistavroupolos, H.D., MacLeod, F.K. and Asmundson, G.J.G., Validation of the Chronic Pain Coping Inventory, Pain, 80 (1999) 471-81. Hagen, K.B., Tambs, K. and Bjerkedal, T., What mediates the inverse association between education and occupational disability from back pain? - A prospective cohort study from the Nord-Trondelag health study in Norway, Social Science & Medicine, 63 (2006) 1267-75. Hamilton, M., A rating scale for depression, Journal Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiat., 23 (1960) 56-61. Hammond, S., Using psychometric tests. In: G.M. Breakwell, S. Hammond and C. Fife-Schaw (Eds.), Research Methods in Psychology, Sage Publications, London, 2000, pp. 175-93. Harris, S., Morley, S. and Barton, S.B., Role loss and emotional adjustment in chronic pain, Pain, 105 (2003) 363-70. Harstall, C. and Ospina, M., How prevalent is chronic pain?, Pain. Clinical Updates, XI (2003) 1-4. Hastie, B., Riley, J.L.I. and Fillingim, R.B., Ethnic differences in pain coping: Factors structure of the Coping Strategies Questionnaire and Coping Strategies Questionnaire Revised, Journal of Pain, 5 (2004) 304-16. Hayes, S.C., Bisset, R.T., Zamir, K., Zettle, R.D., Rosenfarf, I.S., Cooper, L.D. and Grundt, A.M., The impact of acceptance versus control rationales on pain tolerance., The psychological record., 49 (1999) 33-47. Hayes, S.C., Jacobson, N.S., Follete, V.M. and Dougher, M.J., Acceptance and Change: Content and Context in Psychotherapy., Context Press, Reno, 1994. Hayes, S.C., Luoma, J.B., Bond, F.W., Masuda, A. and Lillis, J., Acceptance and Commitment Therapy: Models, processes and outcomes, Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44 (2006) 1-25. Haythornthwaite, J.A., Menefee, L.A., Heinberg, L.J. and Clark, M.R., Pain coping strategies predict perceived control over pain, Pain, 77 (1998) 33-39. 287 Haythornthwaite, J.A., Sieberb, W.J. and Kerns, R.D., Depression and the chronic pain experience, Pain, 46 (1991) 177-84. Helman, C.G., Culture, Health and Illness, Butterworth Heinemann, Oxford, 1996. Henriques, R., Desigualdade e Pobreza no Brasil, IPEA, Rio de Janeiro, 2000. Henry, J.D. and Crawford, J.R., The short form of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scale (DASS-21): Construction validity and normative data in a large nonclinical sample, The British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44 (2005) 22739. Heretik, A. and Molcan, J., A content analysis of depression scales., Activit Nerv Super., 26 (1984) 41-43. Hobara, M., Beliefs about appropriate pain behavior: cross-cultural and sex differences between Japanese and Euro-Americans, European Journal of Pain, 9 (2005) 389-93. Holmbeck, G.N., Toward terminological, conceptual, and statistical clarity in the study of mediators and moderators: examples from the child-clinical and pediatric psychology literature, Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65 (1997) 599-610. Holmes, J.A. and Stevenson, C.A.Z., Differential effects of avoidance and attentional coping strategies on adaptation to chronic and recent-onset pain., Health Psychology, 9 (1990) 577-584. Honeyman, P.T. and Jacobs, E.A., Effects of culture on back pain in Australian aboriginals, Spine, 21 (1996) 841-43. Hosmer, D.W. and Lemeshow, S., Applied Logistic Regression, Wiley and Sons, London, 1989. Hunt, R.R. and Ellis, H.C., Fundamentals of Cognitive Psychology, McGraw-Hill College, USA, 1999. Instituto Brasileiro de Geografia e Estatistica - IBGE, Censo Demográfico - 2000: Características Gerais da População, Instituto Brasileiro de Geografia e Estatistica - IBGE, Rio de Janeiro, 2000. Instituto Brasileiro de Geografia e Estatística - IBGE, Sinopse Preliminar do Censo 2000, Instituto Brasileiro de Geografia e Estatistica - IBGE, Rio de Janeiro, 2000. Jablonska, B., Soares, J.J.F. and Sundin, O., Pain among women: Association with socio-economic and work conditions, European Journal of Pain (2006) 43547. Jacob, M.C., Kerns, R.D., Rosenberg, R. and Haythornthwaite, J.A., Chronic pain: Intrusion and accommodation, Behaviour Research Therapy, 31 (1993) 51927. James, S.J., Large, R.G., Bushnell, J.A. and Wells, J.E., Epidemiology of Pain in New Zealand, Pain, 44 (1991) 279-83. Jamison, R.N., The role of psychological testing and diagnosis in patients with pain. In: R.H. Dworkin and W.S. Breitbart (Eds.), Psychological Aspects of Pain: A handbook for Health Care Providers, Progress in Pain Research and Management, Vol. 27, IASP Press, Seattle, 2004. Jensen, M.P., Questionnaire validation: A brief guide for readers of the research literature., Clinical Journal of Pain (2003) 345-52. Jensen, M.P. and Karoly, P., Control beliefs, coping efforts and adjustment to chronic pain., Journal of Consultation Clinical Psychology, 59 (1991) 431-38. 288 Jensen, M.P., Karoly, P. and Huger, R., The development and preliminary validation of an instrument to assess patients' attitudes toward pain, Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 31 (1987) 393-400. Jensen, M.P., Keefe, F.J., Lefebvre, J.C., Romano, J.J.M. and Turner, J.A., One and two-item measure of pain beliefs and coping strategies., Pain, 14 (2003) 45369. Jensen, M.P., Romano, J.M., Turner, J.A., Good, A.B. and Wald, L.H., Patient beliefs predict patient functioning: further support for a cognitive-behavioural model of chronic pain, Pain, 81 (1999) 95-104. Jensen, M.P., Smith, D.G., Ehde, D.M. and Robinsin, L.R., Pain site and the effects of amputation: further clarification of the meaning of mild, moderate and severe pain, Pain, 91 (2001a) 317-22. Jensen, M.P., Strom, S.E., Turner, J.A. and Romano, J.M., Validity of the Sickness Impact Profile Roland scale as a measure of dysfunction in chronic pain patients, Pain, 50 (1992) 157-62. Jensen, M.P., Turner, J.A. and Romano, J.M., Self-efficacy and outcome expectancies: relationship to chronic pain coping strategies and adjustment, Pain, 44 (1991a) 263-69. Jensen, M.P., Turner, J.A. and Romano, J.M., Correlates of improvement in multidisciplinary treatment of chronic pain, Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62 (1994) 172-79. Jensen, M.P., Turner, J.A. and Romano, J.M., Changes in Beliefs, Catastrophizing, and Coping Are Associated With Improvement in Multidisciplinary Pain Treatment, Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 69 (2001b) 655662. Jensen, M.P., Turner, J.A., Romano, J.M. and Karoly, P., Coping with chronic pain: a critical review of the literature., Pain, 47 (1991b) 249-83. Jensen, M.P., Turner, J.A., Romano, J.M. and Strom, S.E., The chronic pain coping inventory: development and preliminary validation, Pain, 60 (1995) 203-16. Jones, D.A., Rollman, G.B., White, K.P., Hill, M.L. and Brooke, R.I., The Relationship Between Cognitive Appraisal, Affect and Catastrophizing in Patients With Chronic Pain, Journal of Pain, 4 (2003) 267-277. Jorge, M.R., Depression in Brazil and other Latin American countries, Psychiatria et Neurologia Japonica, 105 (2003) 9-16. Keefe, F., Rumble, M.E., Scipio, C.D., Giordano, L.A. and Perri, L.M., Psychological aspects of persisting pain: Current state of the science, The Journal of Pain, 5 (2004) 195-211. Keefe, F.J., Block, A.R. and Williams, R.B., Behavioural treatments of prechronic vs chronic pain patient. In: F.J. Keefe and J.A. Blumenthal (Eds.), Assessment Strategies in Behavioural Medicine, Grune and Stratton, New York, 1990. Keefe, F.J., Lumley, M., Anderson, T., Lynch, T. and Carson, K.L., Pain and emotion: New research directions, Journal of Clinical Psychology, 57 (2001) 587-607. Keefe, F.J. and Williams, D.A., A comparison of coping strategies in chronic pain patients in different age groups., Journal of Gerontology, 45 (1990) 161-65. Kendall, N.A.S., Linton, S., J. and Main, C.J., Guide to Assessing Psychosocial Yellow Flags in Acute Low-Back Pain: Risk Factors for Long-term Disability and Work Loss., Wellington, 1997. 289 Keogh, E., McCracken, L. and Eccleston, C., Gender moderates the association between depression and disability in chronic pain patients, European Journal of Pain, 10 (2006) 413-22. Kerns, R.D., Turk, D.C. and Rudy, T.E., The West haven-Yale Multidimensional Pain Inventory, Pain, 23 (1985) 345-56. Kohlmann, T., Musculoskeletal pain in the population, Der Schmerz, 17 (2003) 40511. Kovacs, F., Abraira, V., Zamora, J., Teresa Gil del Real, M., Llobera, J. and Fernández, C., Correlation between pain, disability, and quality of life in patients with common low-back pain, Spine, 29 (2004) 206-10. Kübler-Ross, E., Living with Death and Dying, Souvenir Press, London, 1982. Lame, I.E., Peters, M.L., Vlaeyen, J.W.S., Kleef, M. and Patijn, J., Quality of life in chronic pain is more associated with beliefs about pain, than pain intensity, Europian Journal of Pain, 9 (2005) 15-24. Lampreia, L.F., Relatório brasileiro sobre desenvolvimento social, Estudos Avançados, 9 (1995) 1-74. Lasch, K.E., Culture and pain, Pain. Clinical Updates, X (2002) 1-6. Lazarus, R.S., Coping theory and research: Past, present and future., Psychosomatic Medicine., 55 (1993) 234-47. Lazarus, R.S. and Folkman, S., Stress, appraisal and Coping., Springer Publishing Company., New York, 1984. Leboeuf-Yde, C., Body weight and low back pain. A systematic literature review of 56 journal articles reporting on 65 epidemiological studies., Spine, 25 (2000) 226-37. Lester, N., Lefebvre, J.C. and Keefe, F.J., Pain in young adults. III. Relationship of three pain-coping measures to pain and activity interference. . Clinical Journal of Pain (1996) 291-300. Linton, S.J., A review of psychological risk factors in back and neck pain, Spine, 25 (2000) 1148-156. Linton, S.J., Do psychological factors increase the risk for back pain in the general population in both a cross sectional and prospective analysis?, European Journal of Pain, 9 (2005) 355-61. Linton, S.J. and Skevington, S.M., Psychological factors. In: I.K. Crombie (Ed.), Epidemiology of Pain, IASP, Seattle, 1999, pp. 25-42. Lipton, J. and Marbach, J., Ethnicity and the pain experience., Soc. Sci. Med. (1984) 1279-97. Loeser, J.D., Perspectives on pain, First World Congress on Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics, London, 1980. Loeser, J.D. and Melzack, R., Pain: an overview, The Lancet, 353 (1999) 1607-09. Lofvander, M.B. and Furhoff, A.K., Backache in Greek immigrants to Sweden. A cultural interpretative approach., Transcultural Psychiatric Research Review (1996) 319-332. Lorencatto, C., Vieira, M.J.N., Pinto, C.L.B. and Petta, C.A., Evaluation of the frequency of depression in patients with endometriosis and pelvic pain, Revista da Associação Médica Brasileira, 48 (2002) 217-21. Lorig, K., Chastain, R.L., Ung, E., Schoor, S. and Holman, H.R., Development and evaluation of a scale to measure perceived self-efficacy in people with arthritis., Arthritis Rheumatology, 32 (1989) 37-44. 290 Lovibond, P.F. and Lovibond, S.H., The structure of negative emotional states: comparison of depression anxiety stress scale (DASS) with the beck depression and anxiety inventories, Behav. Res. Ther., 33 (1995) 335-43. Lovibond, S.H. and Lovibond, P.F., Manual for the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS), Psychology Foundation Monograph, Australia, 1993. Lurie, J., A review of generic health status measures in patients with low back pain, Spine, 25 (2000) 3125-129. Macfarlane, G.J., Who will develop chronic pain and why? The epidemiological evidence, 11th World Congress on Pain, Sydney, 2005. Magnusson, J.E. and Fennell, J.A., Understanding the role of culture in pain: Assessing pain from a Maori perspective, 11th World Congress of Pain, Sydney, 2005. Main, C.J., Robinson, J.P. and Watson, P.J., Disability, incapacity, and rehabilitation for pain patients, 11 th World Congress on Pain, Sydney - Australia, 2005. Marhold, C., Linton, S.J. and Melin, L., Identification of obstacles for chronic pain patients to return to work: Evaluation of a questionnaire, Journal of Occupational Rehabilitation, 12 (2002) 65-75. McCracken, L.M., Behavioral constituents of chronic pain acceptance: Results from factor analysis of the chronic pain acceptance questionnaire, Journal of Back and Musculoskeletal Rehabilitation (1999) 93-100. McCracken, L.M., Carson, J.W., Eccleston, C. and Keefe, F.J., Acceptance and change in the context of chronic pain, Pain, 109 (2004a) 4-7. McCracken, L.M. and Eccleston, C., Coping or acceptance: what to do about chronic pain?, Pain, 105 (2003) 197-204. McCracken, L.M. and Eccleston, C., A prospective study of acceptance of pain and patient functioning with chronic pain, Pain, 118 (2005) 164-69. McCracken, L.M. and Eccleston, C., A comparison of the relative utility of coping and acceptance-based measures in a sample of chronic pain sufferes, European Journal of Pain, 10 (2006) 23-19. McCracken, L.M., Spertus, I.L., Janeck, A.M., Sinclair, D. and Wetzel, F.T., Behavioral dimensions of adjustment in persons with chronic pain: painrelated anxiety and acceptance, Pain (1999) 283-289. McCracken, L.M., Vowles, K.E. and Eccleston, C., Acceptance of chronic pain: component analysis and a revised assessment method, Pain, 107 (2004b) 15066. McDowell, I. and Newell, C., Measuring Health: A Guide to Rating Scales and Questionnaires, Oxford University Press, New York, 1996. McWillians, L.A., Cox, B.J. and Enns, M., Mood and anxiety disorders associated with chronic pain: an examination in a national representative sample, Pain, 106 (2003) 127-33. Melzack, R., Towards a new concept of pain. In: S.D. Waldman and A. Winnie, P. (Eds.), Interventional Pain Management, Saunders Company, Philadelphia, 1996, pp. 1-9. Melzack, R. and Wall, P., Pain Mechanisms: A New Theory, Science, 150 (1965) 971-978. Merskey, H. and Bogduk, N., Classification of Chronic Pain: Descriptions of Chronic Pain Syndromes and Definitions of Pain Terms, IASP Press, Seattle, 1994. 291 Michael, E.S. and Burns, J.W., Catastrophizing and pain sensitivity among chronic pain patients: moderating effects of sensory and affect focus, Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 27 (2004) 185-94. Moore, R. and Brodsgaard, I., Cross-cultural investigations of pain. In: I. Crombie (Ed.), Epidemiology of Pain, IASP, Seattle, 1999. Morley, S. and Keefe, F., Getting a handle on process and change in CBT for chronic pain, Pain, 127 (2007) 197-98. Moulin, D.E., Clark, A.J., Speechley, M. and Morley-Forster, P.K., Chronic pain in Canada - prevalence, treatment, impact and the role of opioid analgesia., Pain Res Manag., 7 (2002) 170-73. Neumann, L. and Buskila, D., Epidemiology of fibromyalgia, Curr Pain Headache Rep., 7 (2003) 362-68. Newton-John, T.R.O., Psychological effects of chronic pain and their assessment in adults. In: T.S. Jensen, P.R. Wilson and A.S.C. Rice (Eds.), Chronic Pain. Clinical Pain Management., Arnold, London, 2003, pp. 101-11. Ng, K.F., Tsui, S.L. and Chan, W.S., Prevalence of common chronic pain in Hong Kong adults, Clinical Journal of Pain, 18 (2002) 275-81. Nguyen, M., Ugarte, C., Fuller, I., Hasas, G. and Portnoy, R., Access to care for chronic pain: racial an ethnic differences, Journal of Pain, 6 (2005) 301-14. Nicholas, M.K., Self-Efficacy and chronic pain, British Psychological Society, London, 1989. Nicholas, M.K., Theory and Practice of Cognitive-Behavioral Programs, Refresher Courses on Pain Management, IASP Press, Seattle, 1996, pp. Pain. An updated review. Nicholas, M.K., The Pain Self-Efficacy Questionnaire: Taking pain into account, European Journal of Pain, 11 (2007) 153-63. Nicholas, M.K. and Asghari, A.M., Investigating acceptance and adjustment in chronic pain: Is acceptance broader than we thought?, Pain, 124 (2006) 26979. Nicholas, M.K. and Molloy, A., Chronic Pain Control: integrating medical and psychosocial treatment option, New Ethicals Journal, December (2002) 6070. Nicholas, M.K., Wilson, P.H. and Goyen, J., Operant-behavioural and cognitivebehavioural treatment for chronic low-back pain, Behavioural Research Therapy, 29 (1991) 225-38. Novello, V.L., Barros, G.A., Danziato, A.M., Cardoso, M.A., Forlim, C.F., Bernardo, T.C., Molina, R.B., Santos, D.C., Cerqueira, A.A. and Lemolina, L., Evaluation of the life quality, anxiety and depression symptoms in a multidisciplinary program for fibromyalgia, 11th World Congress on Pain, Sydney - Australia, 2005. Novy, D.M., Nelson, D.V., Berry, L.A. and Averill, P.M., What does the Beck Depression Inventory measure in chronic pain?: A reappraisal, Pain, 61 (1995) 261-70. Nunnaly, J.C. and Bernstein, I.H., Psychometric Theory, McGraw-Hill, New York, 1994. Nusbaum, L., Natour, J., Ferraz, M.B. and Goldenberg, J., Translation, adaptation and validation of the Roland-Morris Questionnaire - Brazil Roland-Morris, Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research, 34 (2001) 203-10. 292 Osman, A., Barrios, F.X., Kopper, B.A., Hauptmann, W., Jones, J. and O'Neill, E., The pain Catastrophizing Scale: Further psychometric evaluation with an adult sample, Journal of Behavioural Medicine (2000) 351-365. Parliamentary Library., International Comparisons of Economic Size, Living Standards and Price Levels. In: T. Kryger (Ed.), Vol. 2006, Parliament of Australia, Canberra, 2002. Patrick, D.L., Deyo, R.A., Atlas, S.J., Singer, D.E., Chapin, A. and Keller, R.B., Assessing health-related quality of life in patients with sciatica, Spine, 20 (1995) 1899-909. Perls, F., Gestalt Therapy Verbatim, Real People Press, Lafayette, Calififornia, 1969. Picavet, H.S. and Schouten, J.S., Musculoskeletal pain in the Netherlands: prevalences, consequences and risk groups, the DMC (3) - study., Pain, 102 (2003) 167-78. Pimenta, C.A.M., Dor companhia indesejavel., Revista da Fapesp, 65 (2001). Pimenta, C.A.M. and Cruz, D.d.A.L.M., Crenças em dor crônica: validação do Inventário de Atitudes frente à Dor para a Língua Portuguesa.. Revista da Escola de Enfermagem da USP, 40 (2006) 334-343. Pimenta, C.A.M. and Teixeira, M.J., Questionário de dor McGill: proposta de adaptação a língua portuguesa., Revista Brasileira de Reumatologia, 47 (1997) 177-186. Pincus, T., Burton, A.K., Vogel, S. and Field, A.P., A systematic review of psychological factors as predictors of chronicity/disability in prospective cohorts of low back pain, Spine, 27 (2002) 109-20. Pincus, T. and Williams, A., Models and measurements of depression in chronic pain, Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 47 (1999) 211-19. Pincus, T., Williams, A.C.d.C., Vogel, S. and Field, A., The development and testing of the Depression, Anxiety, and Positive Outlook Scale (DAPOS), Pain, 109 (2004) 181-88. Pochmann, M., Educação e trabalho: como desenvolver uma relação virtuosa?, Educação e Sociedade., 25 (2004) 383-99. Portenoy, R.K., Ugarte, C., Fuller, I. and Haas, G., Population-based survey of pain in the United States: differences among White, African American, and Hispanic subjects, The Journal of Pain, 5 (2004) 317-28. Portnoi, A.G., Vasconcellos, E.G., Teixeira, M.J., Pimenta, C.A.M. and Kaziyama, H.H., Transcultural adaption of the Coping Strategies Questionnaire to Brazil: A methodology, 11th World Congress on Pain, Sydney, 2005. Radloff, L., The CED-D scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population, Jour. Appl. Psychol. Meas. (1977) 385-401. Ralphs, J.A., Williams, A.C.d.C., Richardson, P.H., Pither, C.E. and Nicholas, M.K., Opiate reduction in chronic pain patients: a comparison of patient-controlled reduction and staff controlled cocktail methods, Pain, 56 (1994) 279-88. Raspe, H., Matthis, C., Croft, P. and O'Neill, T., Variation in back pain between countries, Spine, 29 (2004) 1017-21. Rey, R., The History of Pain, Harvard University Press, Cambridge, 1998. Riley, J.L., Wade, J.B., Myers, C.D., Sheffiled, D., Papas, R.K. and Price, D.D., Racial/ethnic differences in the experience of chronic pain., Pain, 100 (2002) 291-98. 293 Risdon, A., Eccleston, C., Crombez, G. and McCracken, L.M., How can we learn to live with pain? A Q-methodological analysis of the diverse understanding of acceptance of chronic pain, Social Science & Medicine, 56 (2003) 375-386. Robbins, R.A., Moody, D.S., Hahn, M.B. and Weaver, M.A., Psychological testing variables as predictors or return to work by chronic pain patients, Perceptual & Motor Skills, 8 (1996) 1317-18. Robinson, J.P., Disability evaluation in painful conditions. In: D.C. Turk and R. Melzack (Eds.), Handbook of Pain Assessment, Guilford Press, New York, 2001, pp. 248-72. Robinson, M.E. and Riley III, J.L., The role of emotion in pain. In: R.J. Gatchel and D.C. Turk (Eds.), Psychosocial Factors in Pain: Critical Perspectives., Guilford Press., New York, 1999, pp. 74-88. Robinson, M.E., Riley III, J.L., Sadler, I.J., Kvall, S.A., Geisser, M.E. and Keefe, F.J., The Coping Strategies Questionnaire: A large sample, item level factor analysis., Clinical Journal of Pain, 13 (1997) 43-49. Roland, M. and Fairbank, J., The Roland-Morris Disability Questionnaire and the Oswestry Disability Questionnaire, Spine, 25 (2000) 3115-124. Roland, M. and Morris, R., A study of the natural history of back pain. Part I: Development of a reliable and sensitive measure of disability in low-back pain, Spine, 8 (1983) 141-44. Romano, J.M., Jensen, M.P. and Turner, J.A., The Chronic Pain Coping Inventory42: Reliability and validity., Pain, 104 (2003) 53-73. Romano, J.M. and Turner, J.A., Chronic pain and depression: does the evidence support a relationship., Psychol Bull, 97 (1985) 18-34. Rosenstiel, A.K. and Keefe, F.J., The use of coping strategies in low back pain patients: relationship to patient characteristics and current adjustment., Pain, 17 (1983) 33-44. Rowntree, D., Statistcs Without Tears. A Primer for Non-Mathematicians, Pelican Book, London, 1981. Rudy, T.E., Lieber, S.J.M.S., Boston, J.R., Gourley, L.M.M.A. and Baysal, E.M.S., Psychosocial predictors of physical performance in disable individuals with chronic pain, Clinical Journal of Pain, 19 (2003) 18-30. Ruta, D.A., Hurst, N.P., Kind, P., Hunter, M. and Stubbings, A., Measuring health status in British patients with reumathoid arthritis:reliability, validity and responsiveness of the Short-Form 36-Item Health Survey (SF-36), British Journal of Rheumatology (1998) 425-36. Salvetti, M.d.G. and Pimenta, C.A.M., Validação da Chronic Pain Self-Efficacy Scale para a Língua Portuguesa, Revista de Psiquiatria Clínica, 32 (2005) 202-10. Salvetti, M.G. and Pimenta, C.A.M., Dor crônica e crença de auto-eficácia, Unplished paper (2004). Sardá, J.J.J. and Angelotti, G., Avaliacao Psicologica da Dor. In: J.A.B. Figueiro, G. Angelotti and C.A.M. Pimenta (Eds.), Dor e Saude Mental, Atheneu, Sao Paulo, 2004. Sardá, J.J.J., Nicholas, M.K., Pimenta, C.A.M. and Asghari, A., Psychological Assessment of Chronic Pain Patients: Available Measures in Brazil., 11th World Congress on Pain, Sydney, 2005. 294 Sarda, J.J.J., Pozza, C. and Falavigna, F., Perfil epidemiologico de dores cronicas em academicos do curso de psicologia, VI Simposio Brasileiro e Encontro Internacional sobre Dor, Sao Paulo, 2003. Schultz, I.Z., Crook, J., Meloche, G.R., Berkowitz, J., Milner, R., Zuberbier, O.A. and Meloche, W., Psychosocial factors predictive of occupational low back disability; towards development of a return to work model, Pain (2004) 7785. Severeijns, R., Vlaeyen, J.W.S., van den Hout, M.A. and Picavet, H.S., Pain catastrophizing and consequences of musculoskeletal pain: A prospective study in the Dutch community, Journal of Pain, 6 (2005) 125-32. Sharma, S.C., Singh, R., Sharma, A.K. and Mittal, R., Incidence of low back pain in workage adults in rural North India, Indian J Med Sci., 57 (2003) 145-47. Shaw, W.S., Pransky, G., Patterson, W. and Winters, T., Early disability risk factors for low back pain assessed at outpatients occupational health clinics, Spine, 30 (2005) 572-80. Sheffield, D., Biles, P.L., Orom, H., Maixner, W. and Sheps, D.S., Race and sex differences in cutaneous pain perception, Psychosomatic Medicine, 62 (2000) 517-23. Siddall, P.J. and Cousins, M.J., Introduction to pain mechanisms. Implications for neural blockade. In: M.J. Cousins and P.O. Bridenbaugh (Eds.), Neural Blockade in Clinical Anesthesia and Management of Pain, Lippincott-Raven Publisher, Philadelphia, 1998, pp. 20-33. Siddall, P.J. and Cousins, M.J., Persisting pain as a disease entity: Implications for clinical management, Anesth Analg, 99 (2004) 510-20. Skevington, S.M., Psychology of Pain, John Willey and Sons, England, 1995. Snow-Turek, A.L., Norris, M.P. and Tan, G., Active and passive coping strategies in chronic pain patients., Pain, 64 (1996) 455-62. Stewart, M.W., Harvey, S.T. and Evans, I.M., Coping and catastrophizing in Chronic Pain: A psychometric analysis and comparison of two measures., Journal of Clinical Psychology., 57 (2001) 131-138. Stroud, M.W., McKnight, P.E. and Jensen, M.P., Assessment of self-reported physical activity in patients with chronic pain: Development of an abbreviated Roland-Morris Disability Scale, The Journal of Pain, 5 (2004) 257-63. Stroud, M.W., Thorn, B.E., Jensen, M.P. and Boothby, J.L., The relation between pain beliefs, negative thoughts, and psychosocial functioning in chronic pain patients, Pain, 84 (2000) 347-352. Sullivan, M.E., Thorn, B.E., Haythornthwaite, J.A., Keefe, F.J., Martin, M., Bradley, L.A. and Lefebvre, J.C., Theorethical Perspectives on the Relation Between Catastrophizing and Pain., Clinical Journal of Pain, 17 (2001a) 52-64. Sullivan, M.J.L., Adams, H. and Sullivan, M.E., Communicative dimensions of pain catastrophizing: social cueing effects on pain behaviour and coping, Pain (2004) 220-226. Sullivan, M.J.L., Bishop, S.R. and Pivik, J., The pain catastrophising scale: development and validation., Psychological Assessment, 7 (1995) 524-532. Sullivan, M.J.L. and D'Eon, J.L., Relation between catastrophizing and depression in chronic pain patients, Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99 (1990) 260-263. 295 Sullivan, M.J.L., Feurstein, M., Gatchel, R., Linton, S.J. and Pransky, G., Integrating psychosocial and behavioral interventions to achieve optimal rehabiliation outcomes, Journal of Occupational Rehabilitation, 15 (2005) 475-89. Sullivan, M.J.L., Reesor, K., Mikail, S. and Fisher, R., The treatment of depression on chronic low back pain: Review and recommendations., Pain, 50 (1992) 513. Sullivan, M.J.L., Rodgers, W.M. and Kirsch, I., Catastrophizing, depression and expectancies for pain and emotional distress., Pain (2001b) 147-154. Sullivan, M.J.L., Stanish, W., Waite, H., Sullivan, M.E. and Tripp, D., Catastrophising, pain and disability following soft tissue injuries., Pain, 77 (1998) 253-60. Sullivan, M.J.L. and Stanish, W.D., Pyschologicaly based occupational rehabilitation: The Pain-Disability Prevention Program, Clinical Journal of Pain, 19 (2003) 97-104. Swinkles-Meewisse, I.E., Roelofs, J., Verbeek, A.L.M., Oostendorp, R.A.B. and Vlaeyen, J.W.S., Fear-avoidance beliefs have shown to be associated with disability and work status in patients with low back pain, Clinical Journal of Pain, 22 (2006) 45-54. Tabachnick, B.G. and Fidell, L.S., Using Multivariate Statistics, Allyn and Bacon, New York, 2001. Tait, R.C. and Chibnall, J.T., Racial and ethnic disparities in the evaluation and treatment of pain: psychological perspectives, Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 36 (2005) 595-601. Tan, G., Jensen, M.P., Robinson-Whelen, S., Thornby, J.I. and Monga, T., Measuring control appraisals in chronic pain., Pain, 98 (2002) 385-93. Tan, G., Jensen, M.P., Thornby, J. and Anderson, K.O., Ethnicity, control appraisal, coping, and adjustment to chronic pain among Black and White Americans, Pain Medicine, 6 (2005) 88-98. Tan, V., Cheatle, M.D., Mackin, S., Moberg, P.J. and Esterhai, J.L., Goal setting as a predictor of return to work in a population of chronic musculoskeletal pain patients, International Journal of Neuroscience, 92 (1997) 161-70. Taylor, R., Lovibond, P.F., Nicholas, M.K., Cayley, C. and Wilson, P.H., The utility of somatic items in the assessment of depression in chronic pain patients: A comparison of the Zung Self-Rating Depression Scale (SDS) and the Depression Anxiety Stress Scale (DASS) in chronic pain, clinical sample and community samples, Clinical Journal of Pain, 21 (2005) 91-100. Teasell, R.W. and Bombardier, C., Employment-related factors in chronic pain and chronic pain disability, Clinical Journal of Pain, 17 (2001) 39-45. Thorn, B.E., Ward, L.C., Sullivan, M.J.L. and Boothby, J.L., Communal coping model of catastrophizing: conceptual model building, Pain (2003) 1-2. Turk, D. and Okifuji, A., Clinical assessment of the person with chronic pain. In: T.S. Jensen, P.R. Wilson and A.S.C. Rice (Eds.), Clinical Pain Management, Arnold, London, 2003. Turk, D.C., Biopsychosocial perspective on chronic pain. In: R.J. Gatchel and D.C. Turk (Eds.), Psychosocial Factors in Pain: Critical Perspective, Guilford Press, New York, 1996, pp. 18-34. Turk, D.C., The role of psychological factors in chronic pain, Acta Anaesthesiolol Scand, 43 (1999) 885-88. 296 Turk, D.C., Clinical effectiveness and cost-effectiveness of treatments for patients with chronic pain, The Clinical Journal of Pain (2002a) 355-65. Turk, D.C., A diathesis-stress model of chronic pain and disability following traumatic injury, Pain Res Manage, 7 (2002b) 9-19. Turk, D.C., Meincheinbaun, D. and Genest, M., Pain and Behavioural Medicine: A Cognitive-Behavioural Perspective., Guilford Press, New York, 1983. Turk, D.C. and Monarch, E.S., Biopsychosocial perspectives on chronic pain. In: D.C. Turk and R.J. Gatchel (Eds.), Psychological Approaches to Pain Management, The Guilford Press, New York, 2002, pp. 3-29. Turk, D.C. and Okifuji, A., Psychological factors in chronic pain: Evolution and revolution, Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70 (2002) 678-90. Turk, D.C., Okifuji, A. and Scharff, L., Chronic pain and depression: role of perceived impact and perceived control in different age cohorts, Pain (1995) 93-101. Turner, J.A., Ersek, M. and Kemp, C., Self-efficacy for managing pain is associated with disability, depression and pain coping among retirement community residents with chronic pain, Journal of Pain, 6 (2005) 471-9. Turner, J.A., Fulton-Kehoe, D., Franklin, G., Wickizer, T. and Wu, R., Comparison of the Roland-Morris Disability Questionnaire and Generic Health Status Measures: A Population-Based Study of Workers'Compensation Back Injury Claimants, Spine, 28 (2003) 1061-67. Turner, J.A., Holtzman, S. and Mancl, L., Mediators, moderators, and predictors of therapeutic change in cognitive-behavioral therapy for chronic pain, Pain, 127 (2007) 276-86. Turner, J.A., Jensen, M.P. and Romano, J.M., Do beliefs, coping and catastrophizing independently predict functioning in patients with chronic pain, Pain (2000) 115-125. Turner, J.A. and Leslie, A., Pain-related catastrophizing: What is it?, Clinical Journal of Pain, 17 (2001) 65-71. Turner, J.A. and Romano, J.M., Self-report screening measures for depression in chronic pain patients, Journal Clin Psychology (1984) 909-13. Turp, J.C., Kowalski, C.J. and Stohler, C.S., Greater disability with increased pain involvement, pain intensity and depressive preoccupation, European Journal of Pain, 1 (1997) 271-77. van Leeuwen, M.T., Blyth, F.M., March, L.M., Nicholas, M.K. and Cousins, M.J., Chronic pain and reduced work effectiveness: The hidden cost to Australian employers, European Journal of Pain, 10 (2006) 161-66. Viane, I., Crombez, G., Eccleston, C., Devulder, J. and De Corte, W., Acceptance of the unpleasant reality of chronic pain: Effects upon attention to pain and engagement with daily activities, Pain, 112 (2004) 282-88. Viane, I., Crombez, G., Eccleston, C., Poppe, C., Devulder, J., Van Houdenhove, B. and De Corte, W., Acceptance of pain is an independent predictor of mental weel-being in patients with chronic pain: empirical evidence and reappraisal, Pain, 106 (2003) 65-72. Vlaeyen, J.W. and Linton, S.J., Fear-avoidance and its consequences in chronic musculoskeletal pain: A state of art., Pain, 85 (2000) 317-32. Vlaeyen, J.W.S. and Morley, S., Cognitive-Behavioral Treatments for Chronic Pain. What Works for Whom?, Clinical Journal of Pain, 21 (2005) 1-8. 297 Volinn, E., The epidemiology of low back pain in the rest of the world: A review of surveys in low-and middle income countries, Spine, 22 (1997) 1747-754. Von Korff, M., Epidemiological Methods. In: I.K. Crombie, P.R. Croft, S.J. Linton, L. LeResche and M. Von Korff (Eds.), Epidemiology of pain, IASP Press, Seattle, 1999, pp. 7-16. Von Korff, M., Dworkin, S.F. and Le Resche, L., Graded chronic pain status: an epidemiologic evaluation., Pain, 40 (1990) 279-91. Von Korff, M., Ormel, J., Keefe, F.J. and Dworkin, S.F., Grading the severity of chronic pain, Pain, 50 (1992) 133-49. Von Korff, S.F., LeResche, L.L. and Dworkin, S.F., First onset of common pain symptoms: a prospective study of depression as a risk factor, Pain, 55 (1993) 251-58. Vowles, K.E., Gross, R.T. and Sorrel, J.T., Predicting work status following interdisciplinary treatment for chronic pain, European Journal of Pain (2004) 351-58. Waddell, G., The Back Pain Revolution, Churchill Livingstone, Edinburg, 1998. Waddell, G., The Back Pain Revolution, Churchill Livingstone, Edinburgh, 2003. Wade, J.B., Dougherty, L.M., Archer, C.R. and Price, D.D., Assessing the stages of pain processing: a multivariate analytical approach., Pain, 68 (1996) 157-67. Walsh, N., Schoenfield, l., Ramamurthy, S. and Hoffman, J., Normative model for a cold pressor test., Am J Phys Med Rehabilitation (1989) 6011. Wang, S.J., Epidemiology of migraine and other types of headache in Asia., Curr Neurol Neurosci rep., 3 (2003) 104-8. Ware, J.E., SF-36 Health Survey Update, Spine, 25 (2000) 3130-139. Ware, J.E. and Sherbourne, C.D., The Mos 36-Item Short Form Health Survey (SF36), Medical Care, 30 (1992) 473-81. Watson, P.J., Booker, C.K., Moores, L. and Main, C., Returning the chronically unemployed with low back pain to employment, European Journal of Pain (2004) 359-69. Webb, R., Brammah, T., Lunt, M., Urwin, M., Allison, T. and Symmons, D., Prevalence and predictors of intense, chronic, and disabling neck and back pain in teh UK general population., Spine, 28 (2003) 1195-202. Williams, A.C., The role of psychology in pain management., British Journal of Anaesthesia, 87 (2001) 144-152. Williams, A.C.d.C., Nicholas, M.K., Richardson, P.H., Pither, C.E., Justins, D.M., Chamberlain, J.H., Harding, V.R., Ralphs, J.A., Jones, S.C., Dieudonne, I., Featherstone, J.D., Hodgson, D.R., Ridout, K.L. and Shannon, E.M., Evaluation of a cognitive behavioural programme for rehabilitating patients with chronic pain, British Journal of General Practice, 43 (1993) 513-518. Williams, D.A., Robinson, M.E. and Geisser, M.E., Pain beliefs: assessment and utility, Pain, 59 (1994) 71-78. Williams, D.A. and Thorn, B.E., An empirical assessment of pain beliefs, Pain (1989) 351-58. Woodrow, K.M., Friedman, G.D., Siegelaub, A.B. and Collen, M.F., Pain tolerance: differences according to age, sex and race., Psychosomatic Medicine, 34 (1972) 548-56. World Health Organization, Towards a Common Language for Functioning, disability and Health. 298 The International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health, Geneva, 2002. Worz, R., Pain in depression - Depression in pain, Pain. Clinical Updates, XI (2003) 1-4. Wrubel, J., Benner, P. and Lazarus, R.S., Social competence from the perspective of stress and coping. In: J. Wine and M. Syme (Eds.), Social Competence., Vol. I, Guilford Press, New York, 1981, pp. 173-210. Zatzick, D.F. and Dimsdale, J.E., Cultural variations in response to painful stimuli., Psychosomatic Medicine, 52 (1990) 544-57. Zborowski, M., Cultural components in response to pain, Journal of Social Issues, 8 (1952) 16-31. Zigmond, A.S. and Snaith, R.P., The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale, Acta Psychiat Scand, 67 (1983) 361-70. Zimmermann, M., The history of pain concepts and treatment before IASP. In: H. Merskey, J.D. Loeser and R. Dubner (Eds.), The Paths of Pain, IASP, Seattle, 2005, pp. 1-21. Zung, W.W.K. and Durham, N.C., A self-rating depression scale., Arch Gen Psychiat, 12 (1965) 63-70. 299 Appendix I List of institutions participating in the study 300 301 Appendix II The consent form 302 303 Appendix III Ethic committee approvals 304 305 306 307 308 309 310 311 312 313 Appendix IV Original measures and translations 314 315 316 317 318 319 320 321 322 323 324 325 326 327 Appendix V Publications 328